Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 4/2023

Open Access 11.01.2023 | Original Paper

Can you trust this source? Advice taking in borderline personality disorder

verfasst von: Jakob Scheunemann, Lena Jelinek, Sarah V. Biedermann, Michael Lipp, Amir H. Yassari, Simone Kühn, Jürgen Gallinat, Steffen Moritz

Erschienen in: European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience | Ausgabe 4/2023

Abstract

Research suggests that patients with borderline personality disorder (BPD) share a range of cognitive biases with patients with psychosis. As the disorder often manifests in dysfunctional social interactions, we assumed associated reasoning styles would be exaggerated in a social setting. For the present study, we applied the Judge-Advisor System by asking participants to provide initial estimates of a person’s age and presumed hostility based on a portrait photo. Afterwards, we presented additional cues/advice in the form of responses by anonymous previous respondents. Participants could revise their estimate, seek additional advice, or make a decision. Contrary to our preregistered hypothesis, patients with BPD (n = 38) performed similarly to healthy controls (n = 30). Patients sought the same number of pieces of advice, were equally confident, and used advice in similar ways to revise their estimates. Thus, patients with BPD did trust advice. However, patients gave higher hostility ratings to the portrayed persons. In conclusion, patients with BPD showed no cognitive biases in seeking, evaluating, and integrating socially provided information. While the study implies emotional rather than cognitive biases in the disorder, cognitive biases may still prove to be useful treatment targets in order to encourage delaying and reflecting on extreme emotional responses in social interactions.
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s00406-022-01539-w.

Introduction

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a mental disorder that, according to the DSM-5 [1], has severe symptoms such as suicidal [2, 3] and non-suicidal self-harm behavior [4, 5] as well as impulsive behaviors [6, 7]. Different theories on BPD highlight patients’ abnormalities in terms of how patients process information, draw conclusions, and make sense of the world, especially in the social domain (for a review of theories, see [8]). Understanding the cognitive underpinnings of BPD may help to develop and improve cognitive [9, 10] and metacognitive interventions [11].
BPD patients show a range of reasoning biases [12], such as deficits in problem solving/planning [1315] and decision making [16]. Additionally, comorbidity in patients with BPD is high (especially with depression [17] and PTSD [18]) which may promote additional cognitive biases and cognitive dysfunction [19, 20]. Patients with BPD also self-report elevated levels of jumping to conclusions (JTC) and belief inflexibility [21, 22], which are so-called psychotic-like cognitive biases as they were originally linked to psychosis [23]. JTC, for example, is observed not just in full-blown psychosis but also in remitted psychosis [23], in healthy participants with elevated levels of psychotic-like experiences [24], and even in relatives of patients with psychosis [25, 26]. A linkage of psychosis-like reasoning styles in patients with BPD is compatible with studies reporting that up to 60% of patients with BPD display psychotic symptoms [2729]. However, there is a lack of experimental studies investigating JTC and belief inflexibility in BPD (with only one exception [30]). Of particular interest are reasoning styles within a social setting as BPD core symptoms usually occur in social contexts. For example, the use of information from a social source may be influenced by a trust bias [31, 32]. Our goal in this study was thus to investigate reasoning and decision making in a social context by applying the Judge-Advisor System to patients with BPD.

Applying the Judge-Advisor System to borderline personality disorder

The Judge-Advisor System (JAS; [33]) has mostly been used in social and organizational psychology (for a review, see [34], for a meta-analysis, see [35]) but has also been adapted for research on psychosis [36] and depression [37].
The JAS paradigm consists of a judgment and an advice phase. In the judgment phase, participants make an initial judgment, e.g., estimating the age of a person based on a photograph. In the advice phase, participants receive advice, e.g., in the form of responses by previous respondents. Participants are free to adjust or not to adjust their initial judgment. The main outcome is the degree to which each participant changes their initial judgment in response to advice. Studies with clinical populations found that patients with schizophrenia [36] and depression [37] adjusted their initial estimates more than controls after having received advice. Our group [38] developed the paradigm further; after participants received the first advice, they could either give a final estimate right away or could seek further advice before giving a final estimate. In line with the JTC bias [24], participants with more frequent psychotic-like experiences sought less advice before making a final decision than participants with an average frequency of psychotic-like experiences [38].
The JAS has been demonstrated to be a valuable tool for investigating reasoning and decision making in the social context, specifically how advice is sought and used. For the present study, we are the first to apply the JAS to BPD. Participants made judgments about persons based on portrait photographs of the persons. One such judgment was neutral (estimating the person’s age), and the other judgment was related to BPD symptomology (rating the person’s hostility). BPD patients tend to rate others as, for example, less trustworthy [3941]. After receiving advice in the form of (fabricated) answers of previous respondents, participants could seek additional pieces of advice before making their final decision.
Applying the JAS may inform us about how patients with BPD seek and use advice. This has high practical relevance (e.g., patients with BPD have an increased risk of discharge against medical advice; [42]) and advances our knowledge of cognitive processes in psychiatric disorders (e.g., for a review on recent advancements in predictive processing on the way patients use information to update beliefs, see [43]).

Hypotheses

The JAS paradigm provides measures on information sampling (number of pieces of advice before making a decision), confidence in the decision, and information integration/belief flexibility (adjustment of estimate in response to advice). For each of the three outcomes, we formulated a specific hypothesis (see below).

Information sampling

Preliminary evidence suggests that patients with BPD seek less information before coming to a conclusion. First, patients “jump to conclusions” according to self-report [21, 22]. Second, patients with BPD more often show a JTC bias than healthy controls on the experimental beads task. In this probabilistic reasoning task, patients with BPD sample less information before concluding which of two jars (with different ratios of beads) the instructor is drawing beads from [30]. Also, patients with BPD make riskier (e.g., [44]) and more impulsive decisions [45] than controls. A possible JTC bias might be elevated if the information provided is of a social nature (e.g., in the form of advice from previous respondents), as patients with BPD show a trust bias [31], feel more readily excluded [46], and show increased rejection sensitivity [47, 48]. Furthermore, JTC may represent a transdiagnostic trait, including suicidality and depressiveness [49], both of which patients with BPD are highly prone to [2, 50]. In sum, we assumed that patients with BPD would show a hasty decision-making style (JTC) and thus would seek less advice than controls before giving their final estimate.
H1: Patients with BPD seek less advice than controls.

Confidence

The JAS can also investigate confidence in judgments. Two studies suggest an overconfidence bias for patients with BPD; that is, patients with BPD are more often highly confident in judging emotions when such confidence is not justified [51] or show too high confidence in false memories [52]. Thus, this presumed overconfidence bias should lead to increased confidence in final judgments by patients with BPD compared to controls.
H2: Patients with BPD rate their confidence higher than controls.

Belief flexibility

Advice weighting is the degree the estimates are adjusted in line with advice, which is a measure of belief flexibility. Patients with schizophrenia and participants with elevated levels of psychotic-like-experiences use advice more [36, 38]. As a follow-up study showed, this increased weighting of new information may be explained by aberrant processing of information like hypersalience towards the newly provided advice/information [53]. One can assume similar aberrant processing of information in patients with BPD. Work from computational psychiatry using associative learning tasks suggests that patients with BPD show similar processes to patients with schizophrenia [54], especially in response to social compared to non-social cues [55]. Hence, we expected increased advice weighting among patients with BPD.
H3: Patients with BPD weight advice more than controls.
This study is the first to test the JAS paradigm in patients with BPD. The (adapted) JAS task allows to investigate how patients with BPD evaluate and use socially provided information. As outlined above, we assumed patients with BPD (compared to healthy controls) to seek less advice, to be more confident and to weight advice more.

Methods

Preregistration and ethics

On 20 June 2018 (time-stamped), we non-publicly uploaded our study protocol on AsPredicted (#12037; https://​aspredicted.​org/​xv9fn.​pdf). At that point, data from four patients (10.5%) and six healthy controls (20%) had already been collected but not analyzed. Prior to data collection, the local medical board’s ethics committee approved the trial (trial #PV5263).

Participants and recruitment

We recruited n = 38 in-patients with BPD via the University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf as well as n = 30 healthy controls (HC) via leaflets and word of mouth. Inclusion criteria for patients and healthy controls were (a) age 18–65 years, (b) fluency in German, (c) IQ ≥ 70 as estimated by a vocabulary test, and (d) no neurological disorder. An additional criterion for the clinical sample was a BPD diagnosis according to the DSM-IV, verified with the German version of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-II, [56]). The exclusion criterion was a previous psychotic episode, tested via the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I., [57]). We verified mental health status in controls with the M.I.N.I.; none of the healthy controls reported having previously suffered from any psychological disorder or having sought psychotherapeutic or psychiatric treatment. Samples did not differ according to years of age (BPD: M = 25.7 (SD = 10.6), HC: M = 27.8 (SD = 7.4), t (65.23) = 0.95, p = 0.344) or gender (BPD: 84.2% female, HC: 80.0% female, χ2(1) = 0.205, p = 0.651).

Experimental condition

Estimation task

The procedure followed the standard sequence of a Judge-Advisor System (JAS; [34]), illustrated in Fig. 1. Participants made judgments about four White people (ages 23, 25, 32, and 43; order randomized across participants) based on a portrait photo (displayed with 450 × 338 pixels) taken from the Siblings Database of the CG&V Group [58]. First, participants answered the question “How old is this person?” (Age task) for all four portraits in random order, followed by the question “How hostile is this person?” (Hostility task) with the photos in the same order. Participants entered their estimates on a visual analogue scale (using a slider) with a range from 0 (labeled “not hostile”) to 100 (labeled “hostile”). After participants gave their initial estimates, we showed them the same photos, this time along with advice. In all instructions, we avoided the term “advice.” Instead, the instructions said that they would see randomly selected answers from participants in a pilot study (e.g., “Estimate from participant #99: 45). However, this advice was not real; we computed it, based on each participant’s initial estimates, in the same way for all participants (see Table 1). Advice distances were larger for the Hostility task than for the Age task, as we expected estimating someone’s hostility to be more difficult and thus less precise than estimating someone’s age (as an indication, standard deviations were more than five times larger on the initial hostility than on the initial age estimates, see below). Participants had the opportunity to change the position of the slider and decide whether they wanted to see more answers of previous participants or make a final decision without seeking further advice. After a maximum of four pieces of advice, the trial terminated. In all instances, participants saw all previous estimates and all previous pieces of advice. In each step, participants also rated their confidence on a 4-point scale (1 = unconfident, 2 = rather unconfident, 3 = rather confident, and 4 = 100% confident).
Table 1
Advice in relation to the initial estimate
Sequence
Age Task
Hostility Task
a
b
c
d
a
b
c
d
1st Advice
 + 2
 − 4
 − 6
 + 8
 + 5
 ± 15
 ± 30
 ± 45
2nd Advice
 + 1
 − 3
 − 5
 + 10
 − 7
 ± 7
 ± 20
 ± 58
3rd Advice
 + 3
 − 5
 − 8
 + 7
 − 2
 ± 20
 ± 32
 ± 38
4th Advice
 + 2
 − 4
 − 5
 + 7
 + 4
 ± 18
 ± 38
 ± 39
The order within each sequence (a–d) was fixed, but the allocation to the trial (1–4) was randomized across participants. For sequences b–d for the Hostility task, the advice was in the negative direction for initial estimates > 50 and in the positive direction for initial estimates ≤ 50. The mean advice of all four pieces of advice per sequence equaled the first advice for each sequence, except sequence a in the Hostility task (which equaled 0). Advice was truncated to 0 or 100 for computed advice that resulted in a number < 0 or > 100, respectively. For example, an initial hostility rating of 55 would have resulted in advice for sequence d of 10, 0, 17, and 16, in that fixed order

Scoring

We calculated all scores according to our study protocol. The Number of Requests for Advice (NoRfA) was the number of times a participant requested advice before giving the final estimate (range 0–3), and Confidence was the participant’s confidence rating for the final estimate (range 1–4). As the standard measure for advice integration [34], we calculated Relative Averaged Advice Weighting (RAAW) by dividing the difference between the final and the initial estimate by the difference between the advice and the first estimate (RAAW = [final estimate—initial estimate]/[advice—initial estimate]). When there were multiple pieces of advice, the advice was averaged [59]. In line with our study protocol and previous studies (e.g., [60]), we truncated relative advice-taking scores > 1 to 1 and scores < 0 to 0. We averaged all scores on the subject level separately for both task types (age and hostility) and calculated pooled scores over both tasks.

Psychopathology and procedure

The study design was cross-sectional. Prior to the in-person assessment, participants completed the German version of the 23 item Borderline Symptom List (BSL-23, [61]), the 12-item Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale, the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI, [62]), and the Beck Scale for Suicidal Ideation (19 item version; BSS [63, 64]). During the approximately 2-h long assessment, participants completed the JAS paradigm and the vocabulary test as a proxy for IQ as well as other unrelated experiments as part of a larger study. Further, a trained and supervised rater conducted the M.I.N.I. and rated participants’ depression with the 17-item version of the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HDRS; [65]). At a second approximately half-hour long in-person assessment in the following days, an again trained and supervised rater conducted the SCID-II interview for BPD. For their participation in the entire study, each participant received a reimbursement of 20 Euros.

Preregistered analysis and deviations

We preregistered a sample size of 40 patients with BPD and 40 healthy controls, determined by budget. For two patients, the JAS paradigm was not conducted as the assessments had to be terminated early (resulting n = 38). As this study was part of a larger study, not all healthy controls were included to avoid group differences in age and gender, resulting in n = 30. This decision was made prior to data analysis.
In line with our preregistration, we calculated group differences for the outcome measures NoRfA, Confidence, and RAAW with Welch’s t tests separately for both tasks (age and hostility) as well as for the pooled scores across both tasks. Even though it was not specified in the study protocol, we also report the effect size measure Cohen’s d (|d|< 0.2 negligible, |d|< 0.5 small, |d|< 0.8 medium, |d|≥ 0.8 large) and their corresponding 95% confidence intervals, both calculated with the R package effsize. The mediation analysis—linear relationship between symptom severity (BSL-23) and advice weighting (RAAW), not mediated by self-esteem (RSS)—was calculated according to Baron and Kenny [66], as specified in the preregistration. We calculated the Pearson correlation as correlation coefficients (r), although this was not explicitly specified in the study protocol. We did all analyses using R Studio 1.1.456 [67, 68].

Results

Patients showed severe symptomology (see Tables 2). Borderline symptomology was higher (M = 56.4) than in the BPD validation sample (47.2; [61]). 32 of the 38 patients (84%) with BPD had a BDI-II score of at least 29, indicating severe depression; the remaining 6 patients (16%) had scores indicating moderate depression [62]. According to the M.I.N.I., the great majority of patients (89.5%) had a lifetime depressive disorder, with 28 out of 38 patients (73.7%) currently fulfilling criteria for a depression. About half of the patients had an alcohol-use disorder (55.3%) and/or any non-alcohol substance-use disorder (39.5%); for other disorders according to the M.I.N.I. interview, see Table S1 in the supplementary materials. Compared to controls, patients also had much higher scores on self-reported suicidality (BSS) and depression (HDRS) as well as much lower scores on self-reported self-esteem (RSES). 31 patients were prescribed psychopharmacological treatment at the time of the assessment (81.6%), with an additional four (10.5%) having received psychopharmacological treatment not currently but previously; the remaining three patients (7.9%) had never been medicated.
Table 2
Group differences in psychopathology
 
BPD (n = 38)
HC (n = 30)
t testa
d
95%-CI d
BSL-23
56.4 (17.0)
3.8 (6.7)
t(50.49) = 17.49
3.91
[3.09, 4.74]
BDI-II
37.4 (8.3)
2.8 (4.1)
t(56.61) = 22.54
5.12
[4.12, 6.12]
HDRS
20.2 (5.1)
0.8 (1.6)
t(45.43) = 22.13
4.89
[3.92, 5.86]
BSS
16.1 (8.4)
0.0 (0.2)
t(37.04) = 11.81
2.56
[1.9, 3.2]
RSES
18.1 (4.4)
36.1 (4.5)
t(62.09) = 16.50
4.03
[3.19, 4.88]
CI Confidence interval. BSL-23 Borderline symptom list (23-item version). BDI-II Beck depression inventory-II. HDRS Hamilton depression rating scale. BSS Beck scale for suicidal ideation (19 item version). RSES Rosenberg self-esteem scale
aAll group differences p < 0.001

Judge-Advisor System

All initial age estimates were in the range 24–62 years. Patients and healthy controls estimated with similar accuracy the age of the depicted persons (mean absolute deviation BPD: M = 6.9 (SD = 3.3), HC: M = 7.4 (SD = 3.3); t(61.57) = 0.66, p = 0.513; d = 0.16, 95% CI [− 0.33, 0.65]). Patients rated the persons as more hostile than healthy controls with medium effect size (mean initial hostility rating: BDP: M = 37.8 (SD = 19.1), HC: M = 23.3 (SD = 17.6); t(64.36) = 3.25, p = 0.002; d = 0.79, 95% CI [0.28, 1.29]).

Preregistered results

There were no significant group differences in any preregistered outcome (see Table 3). In comparison to healthy controls, patients with BPD requested negligible (and non-significantly) more advice; rated their confidence slightly (but non-significantly) lower with small effect size; and weighted advice slightly (but non-significantly) stronger with small effect size. This pattern was the same for the Age task and the Hostility task as well as for the pooled average across both tasks. Thus, results did not confirm any of the preregistered main hypotheses.
Table 3
Means and group differences on the main outcomes of the JAS task
 
BPD (n = 38)
HC (n = 30)
t test
p
d
95%-CI d
NoRfA
 Pooled score
0.55 (SD = 0.60)
0.46 (SD = 0.64)
t(60.29) = 0.57
0.572
0.14
[− 0.35, 0.63]
 Age task
0.59 (SD = 0.69)
0.50 (SD = 0.70)
t(61.77) = 0.54
0.589
0.13
[− 0.36, 0.62]
 Hostility task
0.51 (SD = 0.68)
0.43 (SD = 0.62)
t(64.56) = 0.52
0.607
0.12
[− 0.36, 0.61]
Confidence
 Pooled score
2.57 (SD = 0.52)
2.71 (SD = 0.47)
t(64.83) = − 1.19
0.240
− 0.29
[− 0.78, 0.20]
 Age task
2.55 (SD = 0.53)
2.67 (SD = 0.47)
t(65.11) = − 0.99
0.326
− 0.24
[− 0.73, 0.25]
 Hostility task
2.59 (SD = 0.61)
2.75 (SD = 0.55)
t(65.01) = − 1.17
0.247
− 0.28
[− 0.77, 0.21]
RAAW
 Pooled score
0.18 (SD = 0.17)
0.13 (SD = 0.10)
t(62.35) = 1.40
0.168
0.32
[− 0.17, 0.81]
 Age task
0.22 (SD = 0.20)
0.17 (SD = 0.15)
t(65.71) = 1.20
0.234
0.28
[− 0.21, 0.77]
 Hostility task
0.13 (SD = 0.16)
0.09 (SD = 0.11)
t(64.19) = 1.20
0.235
0.28
[− 0.21, 0.77]
CI Confidence interval

Post hoc analysis of main outcomes

As outlined in the supplementary materials S1, we ran a series of post hoc analysis. However, there was no indication for an effect of certain trial types (different distances of advice) and no indication that outliers contributed to the incorrect conclusion of the null-effects. Furthermore, currently depressed patients with BPD in our sample (n = 28, according to the M.I.N.I.) did not significantly differ from healthy controls on any pooled outcome (NoRfA: d = 0.06, p = 0.824; Confidence: d =  − 0.34, p = 0.208; RAAW: d = 0.27, p = 0.316), objecting to a previous finding in which participants with depression had higher advice-taking scores than healthy controls in a JAS task [37].

Correlational analyses

We preregistered an expected relationship between symptom severity (BSL-23) and advice weighting in the patient group. However, there was no indication of a correlation between these two variables (r =  − 0.02, p = 0.898, n = 38). Additionally, symptom severity was neither correlated with NoRfA (r =  − 0.08, p = 0.612, n = 38) nor with Confidence (r = 0.06, p = 0.738, n = 38).
Moreover, no pooled score (NoRfA; Confidence, RAAW) correlated significantly with any other investigated variable in the patient sample (n = 38): Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (r ≤  − 0.31, p ≥ 0.057), Beck Depression Inventory-II (r ≤ 0.10, p ≥ 0.545), HDRS (r ≤ 0.21, p ≥ 0.215), Beck Suicidal Scale (r ≤ 0.14, p ≥ 0.417), or estimated IQ (r ≤|0.26|, p ≥ 0.11). Consequently, and further elaborated in the Supplementary Materials S1, there was no indication of a mediation by self-esteem on the relationship between symptom severity and JAS outcome measures. See Supplementary Materials S1 for correlations between outcomes.

Discussion

In this study, we introduced the Judge-Advisor System (JAS; [33]) to investigate the extent to which patients with BPD seek and use advice. In this task, participants first estimated the age and presumed hostility of a person based on the person’s portrait photo. Second, they received advice in the form of (fabricated) estimates by previous respondents. Participants could subsequently adjust their initial estimate and decide whether to seek additional advice or not. We assumed that patients with BPD would seek less advice, would use advice less to adjust their estimates, and would be more confident in their final estimates compared to healthy controls. Our hypotheses were largely unconfirmed. No significant group differences were found. Hence, the JAS did not reveal any deficits in patients with BPD in their ability to process, seek, or integrate advice by anonymous previous participants.
Our study and hypotheses were prompted by prior research that has linked BPD with cognitive biases initially found in patients with schizophrenia [21, 22], which we assumed would be rather elevated as the information was provided in a social context in which patients with BPD tend to have difficulties. It is thus surprising that patients with BPD did not show deficits on the JAS paradigm, a well-grounded paradigm from social and organizational psychology [34] that has provoked deviant behavior in clinical populations diagnosed with depression [37] and psychosis [36]. In contrast, patients with BPD even performed somewhat better than controls by showing higher accuracy in their initial age estimates, sought somewhat more advice before making a decision, adjusted their estimate slightly more strongly in the direction of the advice (which is generally considered more optimal; [59]), and were less confident (which could be considered functional in view of high ambiguity).
The findings were robust across different experimental manipulations. Results were similar for both the neutral task type (estimating a person’s age) and the borderline-specific task (estimating a person’s hostility). Different distances of advice (difference between advice and initial estimate) did not evoke any group differences. Also, post hoc analysis did not suggest that results were driven by outliers. Moreover, there were no correlations with psychopathological measures on borderline symptomology, depression, or suicidality. Finally, the subgroup of patients with BPD currently fulfilling criteria for depression did not differ from healthy controls (contradicting a previous study by Hofheinz [37]).
In conclusion, our hypotheses were not confirmed. In the context of advice, patients with BPD thus seemed able to use other participants’ advice to improve their answers in an estimation task. In the light of the vast research on the trust bias in BPD [31], it is worth noting that our data do not suggest any primary cognitive bias hampering patients with BPD from correctly evaluating and integrating information coming from other participants.
First, patients did not show a jumping to conclusions bias (JTC bias; [69]) despite the symptom overlap with psychosis [27] and previous indications of a JTC bias in patients with BPD [22, 30]. Even though previous studies using this paradigm did show a JTC bias in psychosis-prone individuals [38], other studies failed to replicate the JTC bias in patients with schizophrenia with novel [70] or even classic paradigms [71]. While some studies have suggested the JTC bias as a transdiagnostic trait [49], evidence from this paradigm does not support this idea. However, concurrent validity between this novel JAS paradigm and classical JTC tasks needs to be further established (for initial evidence, see [38]), especially as healthy controls also sampled a smaller total number of information in the JAS paradigm (i.e. smaller NoRfA) compared to the typically observed total number of information on other JTC tasks, for example the beads task (i.e., Draws to Decision).
Second, regarding confidence, patients with BPD were slightly—but non-significantly—less confident than healthy controls, which again counters one of our hypotheses. Findings thus speak against an overconfidence bias in BPD, which we assumed based on previous findings [51, 52]. On the other hand, results also do not indicate reduced confidence of patients with BPD compared to healthy controls, which was found in other studies (e.g., confidence in judging emotions of others; [72, 73]). Hence, more research is needed to disentangle factors influencing state confidence in patients with BPD. Our study did, however, replicate lower trait self-esteem in patients with BPD compared to healthy controls [74]. However, self-esteem did not correlate with the number of pieces of advice taken or the participant’s confidence and correlated only weakly (but non-significantly) with advice taking. This goes against a previous study in individuals with depression [37] and research from student samples [75].
Third, although we had assumed that patients with BPD would weight advice more than healthy controls, no group differences emerged; patients adjusted their advice marginally more than controls. Hence, in term of belief updating, the data suggest neither a belief inflexibility (previously suggested by Puri et al. [22]) nor an overadjustment/aberrant belief updating (previously suggested by Henco et al. [54]). Instead, the process of belief updating in BPD remains complex [43].
In sum, patients with BPD sought and used anonymous advice the same as healthy controls. This highlights that patients with BPD do not seem to have cognitive biases in evaluating information coming from others. Patients with BPD were not overly hasty and did not naively use information from others (i.e., overadjusting, increased confidence), nor were they overly skeptical (i.e., seeking or using less advice). Instead, patients evaluated their own and others’ skills in the estimation tasks in a healthy manner. In other words, patients with BPD trusted the previous participants’ judgements. Thus, this study also contributes to the vast research on trust in BPD (for a recent review on the trust bias in BPD, see [31]): Interestingly, patients with BPD rated the persons depicted in the photos in the estimation task as more hostile than healthy controls (which is in line with [76, 77]), indicating less trust by patients in others a priori; however, patients trusted the anonymous advice neither more nor less than controls, indicating no diminished trust in the socially provided information. To conclude, the distrust of patients with BPD was not ubiquitous.
To integrate the findings with other research on trust in BPD, we have to note one important aspect of the JAS paradigm used here: participants were given no reason to expect hostile advice as it was given in the form of “randomly selected answers by previous participants.” This means that patients trusted the anonymous advice of a stranger, who had no knowledge about the existence of the patient and who could be expected to answer the estimation question to the best of their knowledge. In that sense, the paradigm resembles studies with little interaction with the trustee. For example, Preuss et al. [78] did not find decreased trust in a one-round trust game in a non-social risk condition in which participants were playing with a computer that was randomly selecting from the actual behavior of previous participants. That study resembles the selection of advice in our study (also “randomly selected”). Thus, it may be less a deficit in trusting others per se (“trust bias”) that drives patient’s behavior and more the fear that a particular individual might exploit or reject the patient. In that sense, patients with BPD might rather show an “untrustworthiness sensitivity” [79], in which they would have a stronger reaction to and expectation of untrustworthy behavior than healthy controls. Whether this reaction is actually deficient per se or, in light of the life history of patients with BPD, even an adaptive behavior stands to debate [80]. Most importantly, results from this study point to the need to be careful when assuming distrust in patients with BPD.
We would like to draw several cautious inferences from the data. Patients with BPD attribute more hostility to others, but this emotional bias is not general; for example, they are not less open to advice than healthy individuals, suggesting that patients can trust others, such as therapists, in some situations. Also, patients did not require additional information or interaction with or about the advisors, suggesting that in anonymous therapeutic settings like online interventions, patients might be open to advice. To disentangle this, future studies should manipulate information about the advisor (e.g., an expert giving advice) and see whether the finding translates to therapeutic content.
This study also adds to the field of cognitive bias research on BPD. For schizophrenia, basic research has identified specific cognitive biases that are successfully targeted in interventions, such as metacognitive training [81]. Tentative evidence from metacognitive training adapted for patients with BPD (B-MCT) shows that modifying biases may indeed slow decision-making and ameliorate symptoms [11]. In light of the present findings, however, the magnitude of cognitive distortions seems to be weaker in BPD than in schizophrenia. Thus, the B-MCT’s modules on JTC and memory/overconfidence should be reconsidered critically. Yet, while a hasty decision making style might not be a core BPD feature, patients with BPD might still benefit from improved metacognitive skills to counter frequent over-reactions and one-sided judgments typically seen in the disorder. This study suggests that metacognitive interventions can draw on patients’ high metacognitive capacity to help patients to access these skills more readily in highly arousing, real-life situations.
The study has some strengths, in our opinion. The analyses were preregistered, which by some experts is regarded as an important criterion for good scientific practice [82], and the range of manipulations of the JAS paradigm speaks for the robustness of the detected results. However, as in most clinical studies, a clinical comparison group is missing and sample sizes were small. A larger sample would have possibly allowed to investigate whether certain biases are associated with specific BPD features not present in all patients. While patients and controls did not differ on the core sociodemographic variables age and gender, subsample sizes were not balanced. Possibly, our recruitment was somewhat biased as we included only currently hospitalized patients who are help-seeking and thus may be more open to seeking advice than non-hospitalized patients. Previous or current treatment could have also influenced patients’ behavior (however, we tried to included participants as shortly as possible after admission). Also, psychiatric symptomology in our patient sample was above average, which goes along with factors that may exert an effect on cognitive biases like psychopharmacological treatment [83, 84], comorbidity, and psychosocial factors like lower socioeconomic status [85]. Future studies should manipulate aspects of the given advice more rigorously, such as by showing multiple pieces of advice and providing more information about the advisor. Other adjustments to the paradigm could be to use other estimates than age or to change the scales (e.g. using probability estimates as a confidence scale). Future studies might also want to consider race in the study design and analyses.

Conclusion

Applying the JAS paradigm from social psychology, we investigated how patients with BPD seek and use (socially provided) information. In two estimation tasks, patients with BPD did not behave differently compared to healthy controls. Patients sought the same number of pieces of advice before submitting their final estimate, were similarly confident in their final estimate, and adjusted their initial estimate equally in response to the advice. This contradicted our preregistered hypotheses. These results suggest that the lack of trust in others observed in patients with BPD may not be driven by cognitive biases but rather by other factors such as emotional biases (e.g., rejection sensitivity). This raises the hope that patients with BPD may be able to deal with advice the same way healthy individuals do, for example, when receiving advice within a therapeutic setting or in systemic therapy approaches like family therapy.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors have no competing interests to declare that are relevant to the content of this article. No funding was received to assist with the preparation of this manuscript.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Jetzt e.Med zum Sonderpreis bestellen!

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Jetzt bestellen und 100 € sparen!

Neuer Inhalt

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Weitere Produktempfehlungen anzeigen
Anhänge

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association DS, Association AP et al (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. American psychiatric association, Washington, DCCrossRef American Psychiatric Association DS, Association AP et al (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5. American psychiatric association, Washington, DCCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Storebø OJ, Stoffers-Winterling JM, Völlm BA, Kongerslev MT, Mattivi JT, Jørgensen MS, Faltinsen E, Todorovac A, Sales CP, Callesen HE, Lieb K, Simonsen E (2020) Psychological therapies for people with borderline personality disorder. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 5:CD012955PubMed Storebø OJ, Stoffers-Winterling JM, Völlm BA, Kongerslev MT, Mattivi JT, Jørgensen MS, Faltinsen E, Todorovac A, Sales CP, Callesen HE, Lieb K, Simonsen E (2020) Psychological therapies for people with borderline personality disorder. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 5:CD012955PubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Schilling L, Moritz S, Kriston L, Krieger M, Nagel M (2018) Efficacy of metacognitive training for patients with borderline personality disorder: Preliminary results. Psychiatry Res 262:459–464CrossRefPubMed Schilling L, Moritz S, Kriston L, Krieger M, Nagel M (2018) Efficacy of metacognitive training for patients with borderline personality disorder: Preliminary results. Psychiatry Res 262:459–464CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Ruocco AC (2005) The neuropsychology of borderline personality disorder: a meta-analysis and review. Psychiatry Res 137:191–202CrossRefPubMed Ruocco AC (2005) The neuropsychology of borderline personality disorder: a meta-analysis and review. Psychiatry Res 137:191–202CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Ford JD, Courtois CA (2021) Complex PTSD and borderline personality disorder. Border Person. Dis. Emot. Dysregul. 8:1–21 Ford JD, Courtois CA (2021) Complex PTSD and borderline personality disorder. Border Person. Dis. Emot. Dysregul. 8:1–21
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Unoka Z, Richman MJ (2016) Neuropsychological deficits in BPD patients and the moderator effects of co-occurring mental disorders: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 44:1–12CrossRefPubMed Unoka Z, Richman MJ (2016) Neuropsychological deficits in BPD patients and the moderator effects of co-occurring mental disorders: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 44:1–12CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Choate AM, Fatimah H, Bornovalova MA (2021) Comorbidity in borderline personality: understanding dynamics in development. Curr Opin Psychol 37:104–108CrossRefPubMed Choate AM, Fatimah H, Bornovalova MA (2021) Comorbidity in borderline personality: understanding dynamics in development. Curr Opin Psychol 37:104–108CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Moritz S, Schilling L, Wingenfeld K, Köther U, Wittekind C, Terfehr K, Spitzer C (2011) Psychotic-like cognitive biases in borderline personality disorder. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 42:349–354CrossRefPubMed Moritz S, Schilling L, Wingenfeld K, Köther U, Wittekind C, Terfehr K, Spitzer C (2011) Psychotic-like cognitive biases in borderline personality disorder. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 42:349–354CrossRefPubMed
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Henquet C, van Os J, Pries LK, Rauschenberg C, Delespaul P, Kenis G, Luykx JJ, Lin BD, Richards AL, Akdede B et al (2020) A replication study of JTC bias, genetic liability for psychosis and delusional ideation. Psychol Med 52(9):1–7 Henquet C, van Os J, Pries LK, Rauschenberg C, Delespaul P, Kenis G, Luykx JJ, Lin BD, Richards AL, Akdede B et al (2020) A replication study of JTC bias, genetic liability for psychosis and delusional ideation. Psychol Med 52(9):1–7
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Dael F, Versmissen D, Janssen I, Myin-Germeys I, Van Os J, Krabbendam L (2006) Data gathering: biased in psychosis? Schizophr Bull 32:341–351CrossRefPubMed Van Dael F, Versmissen D, Janssen I, Myin-Germeys I, Van Os J, Krabbendam L (2006) Data gathering: biased in psychosis? Schizophr Bull 32:341–351CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Bonaccio S, Dalal RS (2006) Advice taking and decision-making: an integrative literature review, and implications for the organizational sciences. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 101:127–151CrossRef Bonaccio S, Dalal RS (2006) Advice taking and decision-making: an integrative literature review, and implications for the organizational sciences. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 101:127–151CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Hofheinz C, Germar M, Schultze T, Michalak J, Mojzisch A (2017) Are depressed people more or less susceptible to informational social influence? Cognit Ther Res 41:699–711CrossRef Hofheinz C, Germar M, Schultze T, Michalak J, Mojzisch A (2017) Are depressed people more or less susceptible to informational social influence? Cognit Ther Res 41:699–711CrossRef
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Hepp J, Kieslich PJ, Schmitz M, Schmahl C, Niedtfeld I (2021) Negativity on two sides: individuals with borderline personality disorder form negative first impressions of others and are perceived negatively by them. Personal Disord Theory, Res Treat. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000412CrossRef Hepp J, Kieslich PJ, Schmitz M, Schmahl C, Niedtfeld I (2021) Negativity on two sides: individuals with borderline personality disorder form negative first impressions of others and are perceived negatively by them. Personal Disord Theory, Res Treat. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​per0000412CrossRef
48.
Zurück zum Zitat Cavicchioli M, Maffei C (2020) Rejection sensitivity in borderline personality disorder and the cognitive–affective personality system: a meta-analytic review. Personal Disord Theory, Res Treat 11:1CrossRef Cavicchioli M, Maffei C (2020) Rejection sensitivity in borderline personality disorder and the cognitive–affective personality system: a meta-analytic review. Personal Disord Theory, Res Treat 11:1CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Sastre-Buades A, Ochoa S, Lorente-Rovira E, Barajas A, Grasa E, López-Carrilero R, Luengo A, Ruiz-Delgado I, Cid J, González-Higueras F, Sánchez-Alonso S, Baca-García E, Barrigón ML, Acevedo A, Anglès J, Argany MA, Barajas A, Barrigón ML, Beltrán M, Birulés I, Bogas JL, Camprubí N, Carbonero M, Carmona Farrés C, Carrasco E, Casañas R, Cid J, Conesa E, Corripio I, Cortes P, Crosas JM, de Apraiz A, Delgado M, Domínguez L, Escartí MJ, Escudero A, Esteban Pinos I, Figueras M, Franco C, García C, Gil V, Giménez-Díaz D, Gonzalez-Casares R, González Higueras F, González- Montoro ML, González E, Grasa Bello E, Guasp A, Huerta-Ramos ME, Huertas P, Jiménez-Díaz A, Lalucat LL, Llacer B, López-Alcayada R, López- Carrilero R, Lorente E, Luengo A, Mantecón N, Mas-Expósito L, Montes M, Moritz S, Murgui E, Nuñez M, Ochoa S, Palomer E, Paniego E, Peláez T, Pérez V, Planell K, Planellas C, Pleguezuelo-Garrote P, Pousa E, Rabella M, Renovell M, Rubio R, Ruiz- Delgado I, San Emeterio M, Sánchez E, Sanjuán J, Sans B, Schilling L, Sió H, Teixidó M, Torres P, Vila MA, Vila-Badia R, Villegas F, Villellas R (2021) Jumping to conclusions and suicidal behavior in depression and psychosis. J Psychiatr Res 137:514–520. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.03.024CrossRefPubMed Sastre-Buades A, Ochoa S, Lorente-Rovira E, Barajas A, Grasa E, López-Carrilero R, Luengo A, Ruiz-Delgado I, Cid J, González-Higueras F, Sánchez-Alonso S, Baca-García E, Barrigón ML, Acevedo A, Anglès J, Argany MA, Barajas A, Barrigón ML, Beltrán M, Birulés I, Bogas JL, Camprubí N, Carbonero M, Carmona Farrés C, Carrasco E, Casañas R, Cid J, Conesa E, Corripio I, Cortes P, Crosas JM, de Apraiz A, Delgado M, Domínguez L, Escartí MJ, Escudero A, Esteban Pinos I, Figueras M, Franco C, García C, Gil V, Giménez-Díaz D, Gonzalez-Casares R, González Higueras F, González- Montoro ML, González E, Grasa Bello E, Guasp A, Huerta-Ramos ME, Huertas P, Jiménez-Díaz A, Lalucat LL, Llacer B, López-Alcayada R, López- Carrilero R, Lorente E, Luengo A, Mantecón N, Mas-Expósito L, Montes M, Moritz S, Murgui E, Nuñez M, Ochoa S, Palomer E, Paniego E, Peláez T, Pérez V, Planell K, Planellas C, Pleguezuelo-Garrote P, Pousa E, Rabella M, Renovell M, Rubio R, Ruiz- Delgado I, San Emeterio M, Sánchez E, Sanjuán J, Sans B, Schilling L, Sió H, Teixidó M, Torres P, Vila MA, Vila-Badia R, Villegas F, Villellas R (2021) Jumping to conclusions and suicidal behavior in depression and psychosis. J Psychiatr Res 137:514–520. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​jpsychires.​2021.​03.​024CrossRefPubMed
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Dubo ED, Sickel AE, Trikha A, Levin A, Reynolds V (1998) Axis I comorbidity of borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 155:1733–1739CrossRefPubMed Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Dubo ED, Sickel AE, Trikha A, Levin A, Reynolds V (1998) Axis I comorbidity of borderline personality disorder. Am J Psychiatry 155:1733–1739CrossRefPubMed
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Scheunemann J, Fischer R, Moritz S (2021) Probing the hypersalience hypothesis—an adapted judge-advisor system tested in individuals with psychotic-like experiences. Front psychiatry 12:158CrossRef Scheunemann J, Fischer R, Moritz S (2021) Probing the hypersalience hypothesis—an adapted judge-advisor system tested in individuals with psychotic-like experiences. Front psychiatry 12:158CrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Henco L, Diaconescu AO, Lahnakoski JM, Brandi ML, Hörmann S, Hennings J, Hasan A, Papazova I, Strube W, Bolis D, Schilbach L, Mathys C (2020) Aberrant computational mechanisms of social learning and decision-making in schizophrenia and borderline personality disorder. PLoS Comput Biol 16:1–22. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008162CrossRef Henco L, Diaconescu AO, Lahnakoski JM, Brandi ML, Hörmann S, Hennings J, Hasan A, Papazova I, Strube W, Bolis D, Schilbach L, Mathys C (2020) Aberrant computational mechanisms of social learning and decision-making in schizophrenia and borderline personality disorder. PLoS Comput Biol 16:1–22. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1371/​journal.​pcbi.​1008162CrossRef
56.
Zurück zum Zitat First MB, Gibbon M (2004) The structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders (SCID-I) and the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis II disorders (SCID-II). In: Hilsenroth MJ, Segal DL (eds) Comprehensive handbook of psychological assessment. John Wiley & Sons Inc, pp 134–143 First MB, Gibbon M (2004) The structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders (SCID-I) and the structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis II disorders (SCID-II). In: Hilsenroth MJ, Segal DL (eds) Comprehensive handbook of psychological assessment. John Wiley & Sons Inc, pp 134–143
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC (1998) The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry 59:22–33PubMed Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, Hergueta T, Baker R, Dunbar GC (1998) The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry 59:22–33PubMed
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Yaniv I, Milyavsky M (2007) Using advice from multiple sources to revise and improve judgments. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 103:104–120CrossRef Yaniv I, Milyavsky M (2007) Using advice from multiple sources to revise and improve judgments. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process 103:104–120CrossRef
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Soll JB, Larrick RP (2009) Strategies for revising judgment: how (and how well) people use others’ opinions. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 35:780CrossRefPubMed Soll JB, Larrick RP (2009) Strategies for revising judgment: how (and how well) people use others’ opinions. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 35:780CrossRefPubMed
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Bohus M, Kleindienst N, Limberger MF, Stieglitz R-D, Domsalla M, Chapman AL, Steil R, Philipsen A, Wolf M (2009) The short version of the borderline symptom list (BSL-23): development and initial data on psychometric properties. Psychopathology 42:32–39. https://doi.org/10.1159/000173701CrossRefPubMed Bohus M, Kleindienst N, Limberger MF, Stieglitz R-D, Domsalla M, Chapman AL, Steil R, Philipsen A, Wolf M (2009) The short version of the borderline symptom list (BSL-23): development and initial data on psychometric properties. Psychopathology 42:32–39. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1159/​000173701CrossRefPubMed
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Beck A, Steer R, Brown G (1996) Manual for the beck depression inventory-II. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio Beck A, Steer R, Brown G (1996) Manual for the beck depression inventory-II. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Baron RM, Kenny DA (1986) The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51:1173CrossRefPubMed Baron RM, Kenny DA (1986) The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51:1173CrossRefPubMed
67.
Zurück zum Zitat RStudio Team (2015) RStudio: integrated development environment for R RStudio Team (2015) RStudio: integrated development environment for R
68.
Zurück zum Zitat R Core Team (2014) R: a language and environment for statistical computing R Core Team (2014) R: a language and environment for statistical computing
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Tripoli G, Quattrone D, Ferraro L, Gayer-Anderson C, Rodriguez V, La Cascia C, La Barbera D, Sartorio C, Seminerio F, Tarricone I et al (2020) Jumping to conclusions, general intelligence, and psychosis liability: findings from the multi-centre EU-GEI case-control study. Psychol Med 51(4):1–11 Tripoli G, Quattrone D, Ferraro L, Gayer-Anderson C, Rodriguez V, La Cascia C, La Barbera D, Sartorio C, Seminerio F, Tarricone I et al (2020) Jumping to conclusions, general intelligence, and psychosis liability: findings from the multi-centre EU-GEI case-control study. Psychol Med 51(4):1–11
73.
Zurück zum Zitat Thome J, Liebke L, Bungert M, Schmahl C, Domes G, Bohus M, Lis S (2016) Confidence in facial emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder. Personal Disord Theory Res Treat 7:159CrossRef Thome J, Liebke L, Bungert M, Schmahl C, Domes G, Bohus M, Lis S (2016) Confidence in facial emotion recognition in borderline personality disorder. Personal Disord Theory Res Treat 7:159CrossRef
74.
Zurück zum Zitat Winter D, Bohus M, Lis S (2017) Understanding negative self-evaluations in borderline personality disorder—a review of self-related cognitions, emotions, and motives. Curr Psychiatry Rep 19(3):1–9CrossRef Winter D, Bohus M, Lis S (2017) Understanding negative self-evaluations in borderline personality disorder—a review of self-related cognitions, emotions, and motives. Curr Psychiatry Rep 19(3):1–9CrossRef
77.
Zurück zum Zitat Richetin J, Poggi A, Ricciardelli P, Fertuck EA, Preti E (2018) The emotional components of rejection sensitivity as a mediator between borderline personality disorder and biased appraisal of trust in faces. Clin Neuropsychiatry 15:200–205 Richetin J, Poggi A, Ricciardelli P, Fertuck EA, Preti E (2018) The emotional components of rejection sensitivity as a mediator between borderline personality disorder and biased appraisal of trust in faces. Clin Neuropsychiatry 15:200–205
81.
Zurück zum Zitat Moritz S, Woodward TS (2007) Metacognitive training in schizophrenia: from basic research to knowledge translation and intervention. Curr Opin Psychiatry 20:619–625CrossRefPubMed Moritz S, Woodward TS (2007) Metacognitive training in schizophrenia: from basic research to knowledge translation and intervention. Curr Opin Psychiatry 20:619–625CrossRefPubMed
83.
Zurück zum Zitat Andreou C, Moritz S, Veith K, Veckenstedt R, Naber D (2013) Dopaminergic modulation of probabilistic reasoning and overconfidence in errors: a double-blind study. Schizophr Bull 40:558–565CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Andreou C, Moritz S, Veith K, Veckenstedt R, Naber D (2013) Dopaminergic modulation of probabilistic reasoning and overconfidence in errors: a double-blind study. Schizophr Bull 40:558–565CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
84.
Zurück zum Zitat Andreou C, Schneider BC, Braun V, Kolbeck K, Gallinat J, Moritz S (2015) Dopamine effects on evidence gathering and integration. J Psychiatry Neurosci 40:140306CrossRef Andreou C, Schneider BC, Braun V, Kolbeck K, Gallinat J, Moritz S (2015) Dopamine effects on evidence gathering and integration. J Psychiatry Neurosci 40:140306CrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Can you trust this source? Advice taking in borderline personality disorder
verfasst von
Jakob Scheunemann
Lena Jelinek
Sarah V. Biedermann
Michael Lipp
Amir H. Yassari
Simone Kühn
Jürgen Gallinat
Steffen Moritz
Publikationsdatum
11.01.2023
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience / Ausgabe 4/2023
Print ISSN: 0940-1334
Elektronische ISSN: 1433-8491
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-022-01539-w

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 4/2023

European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 4/2023 Zur Ausgabe

Hörschwäche erhöht Demenzrisiko unabhängig von Beta-Amyloid

29.05.2024 Hörstörungen Nachrichten

Hört jemand im Alter schlecht, nimmt das Hirn- und Hippocampusvolumen besonders schnell ab, was auch mit einem beschleunigten kognitiven Abbau einhergeht. Und diese Prozesse scheinen sich unabhängig von der Amyloidablagerung zu ereignen.

So wirken verschiedene Alkoholika auf den Blutdruck

23.05.2024 Störungen durch Alkohol Nachrichten

Je mehr Alkohol Menschen pro Woche trinken, desto mehr steigt ihr Blutdruck, legen Daten aus Dänemark nahe. Ob es dabei auch auf die Art des Alkohols ankommt, wurde ebenfalls untersucht.

„Übersichtlicher Wegweiser“: Lauterbachs umstrittener Klinik-Atlas ist online

17.05.2024 Klinik aktuell Nachrichten

Sie sei „ethisch geboten“, meint Gesundheitsminister Karl Lauterbach: mehr Transparenz über die Qualität von Klinikbehandlungen. Um sie abzubilden, lässt er gegen den Widerstand vieler Länder einen virtuellen Klinik-Atlas freischalten.

ADHS-Medikation erhöht das kardiovaskuläre Risiko

16.05.2024 Herzinsuffizienz Nachrichten

Erwachsene, die Medikamente gegen das Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-Hyperaktivitätssyndrom einnehmen, laufen offenbar erhöhte Gefahr, an Herzschwäche zu erkranken oder einen Schlaganfall zu erleiden. Es scheint eine Dosis-Wirkungs-Beziehung zu bestehen.

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.