Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics 1/2023

Open Access 01.12.2023 | Research

Development and validation of a self-regulation scale within the German version of the Early Development Instrument

verfasst von: Sabine Georg, Bernd Genser, Joachim Fischer, Steffi Sachse, Freia De Bock

Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics | Ausgabe 1/2023

Abstract

Background

Early childhood self-regulation (SR) is key for many health- and education-related outcomes across the life span. Kindergarten age is a crucial period for SR development, and within this developmental window, potential SR difficulties can still be compensated for (e.g., through interventions). However, efficient measurement of SR through brief, comprehensive, and easy-to-use instruments that identify SR difficulties are scarce. To address this need, we used items of an internationally applied kindergarten teacher questionnaire—the Early Development Instrument (EDI) – to develop and validate a specific SR measurement scale.

Methods

The psychometric evaluation and validation of the selected SR-items was performed in data collected with the German version of the EDI (GEDI), in two independent data sets – (a) the development dataset, with 191 children, and b) the validation dataset, with 184 children. Both included three- to six-year-old children and contained retest and interrater reliability data. First, three independent raters—based on theory—selected items eligible to form a SR scale from the two SR-relevant GEDI domains "social competence" and "emotional maturity". Second, exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis using structural equation modeling examined the item structure across both data sets. This resulted in a defined SR scale, of which internal consistency, test–retest and interrater reliability, cross-validation, and concurrent validity using correlation and descriptive agreements (Bland–Altman (BA) plots) with an existing validated SR-measuring instrument (the Kindergarten Behavioral Scales) were assessed.

Results

Confirmatory factor analysis across both data sets yielded the best fit indices with 13 of the GEDI 20 items initially deemed eligible for SR measurement, and a three-factor structure: a) behavioral response inhibition, b) cognitive inhibition, c) selective or focused attention (RMSEA: 0.019, CFI: 0.998). Psychometric evaluation of the resulting 13-item-GEDI-SR scale revealed good internal consistency (0.92), test–retest and interrater reliability (0.85 and 0.71, respectively), validity testing yielded stability across populations and good concurrent validity with the Kindergarten Behavioral Scales (Pearson correlation coefficient: mean 0.72, range 0.61 to 0.84).

Conclusions

The GEDI contains 13 items suitable to assess SR, either as part of regular EDI developmental monitoring or as a valid stand-alone scale. This short 13-item (G)EDI-SR scale may allow early detection of children with SR difficulties in the kindergarten setting in future and could be the basis for public health intervention planning. To attain this goal, future research should establish appropriate reference values using a representative standardization sample.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
BA
Bland-Altman
BIKO
Bildungskompetenzen Organisieren / BIKO-Screening zur Entwicklung von Basiskompetenzen für 3- bis 6-Jährige (Organizing Education in Preschool)
BRIEF-P
Behavior rating inventory of executive function—preschool version
CFI
Comparative fit index
CPD
Center for Preventive Medicine and Digital Health
DESK 3-6
Dortmunder Entwicklungs Screening für den Kindergarten (Dortmund Developmental Screening for Kindergarten)
GEDI
German version of the Early Development Instrument
GEDI-SR scale
GEDI self-regulation scale
ICC
Intraclass correlation coefficient
KOMPIK
Kompetenzen und Interessen von Kindern (Competencies and Interests of Children)
RMSEA
Root mean squared error of approximation
SDQ
Strengths and difficulties questionnaire
SES
Socioeconomic status
SR
Self-regulation
VSK
Verhaltenssalen für das Kindergartenalter (Kinergarten Behavior Scales)
VSK-SR
VSK self-regulation scale

Introduction

Self regulation (SR) is a fundamental developmental skill impacting a child’s performance and health across the lifespan [1, 2]. It describes the ability to adapt one's thoughts, feelings, and behavior to the demands of a particular situation in order to optimally pursue personal goals [3]. Moreover, SR refers to processes that enable us to maintain optimal levels of emotional, motivational, and cognitive arousal. It […] overlaps substantially with inhibitory control, a core dimension of executive functions [4].
From a medical, psychological and pedagogical perspective, good SR skills are considered a protective factor regarding mental [57] and physical health [8] and have been found to longitudinally predict health, success in professional and private life, satisfaction with life and social equity in adulthood [1].
Accumulating evidence in the last two decades suggests that more and more children from school age to adolescence have difficulties in regulating their behaviors [9]. For example, the prevalence of behavioral and psychological problems related to SR in kindergarten and primary school has been steadily increasing [2, 1012]. This not only presents challenges for the daily work of teachers [1315], but studies also suggest that these problems persist into adolescence with a 50% chance [16], resulting in a high societal burden and possible medical costs [17, 18].
With the window for promoting children’s SR skills opening years before entering school, early identification of children with SR difficulties combined with early intervention e.g. in kindergarten seems key from a public health perspective. As SR development depends on environmental factors and experiences [1921] (besides biological maturity), interventions that change the environment and experiences have the potential to effectively support child SR development [2224]. Current systematic reviews have shown effectiveness of different SR promoting interventions in early childhood education and care environments (ECECs) [23, 24]. Other studies showed that supportive environmental factors such as high-quality teacher–child interaction [25] are positively associated with SR development in children. This suggests that a public health approach combining the efficient identification of children with SR difficulties early on with the implementation of effective interventions in the kindergarten setting has a high potential.
To identify vulnerable children, valid measurement of SR in kindergartens is necessary. As SR skills are part of psychological and social-emotional child development, questionnaires that are used to assess the latter might be promising. These include the Behavioral and Emotional Rating Scale (BERS, 26 items, domains: behavioral self-control, emotional self-control) [26], the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL, 33 items, domains: emotionally reactive, attention problems, aggressive behavior) [27], the Child Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ, 12 items, domains: attentional focusing, inhibitory control) [28], the Child Behavior Rating Scale (CBRS, 17 items, domains: self-regulation, social/interpersonal skills) [29], Conners' rating scale – teacher form (CTRS, 28 items, domains: conduct problems, day-dreaming inattention, anxious fearful, hyperactivity) [30], the Devereux Early Childhood Assessment (DECA, 8 items, domain: self-control) [31], Social competence and behavior evaluation—preschool edition (SCBE, 20 items, domains: anger-aggression, social competence) [32], the Social Competence Scale (SCS, 13 items, domains: prosocial behavior, emotion regulation) [33], the Strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ, 25 items, domains: emotional symptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems, prosocial behavior) [34, 35], and the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Preschool Version (BRIEF-P, 63 items, domains: inhibition, attention shift, emotional control, working memory, planning/organizing) [36]. Although many instruments might be available to measure SR skills, the most important ones were suggested to be the CBQ, BRIEF, CBCL and SDQ [37]. However, from a public health perspective, all of these are too comprehensive and long (e.g. number of items for SR measurement = 12, 26 23, 25, respectively) for screening purposes, and do not feature SR as a separate construct.
Several of these questionnaires also exist in German, e.g. the SDQ or the BRIEF-P [38]. Furthermore, additional questionnaires exist that were developed in the German context and are primarily used in Germany, such as the Kindergarten Behavior Scales (VSK, 49 items, domains: anxiety, hyperactivity and inattention, aggressive behavior, emotional dysregulation, social competence, emotional knowledge/empathy, self-regulation) [39], the Organizing Education in Kindergarten screening (BIKO, 33 items six domains: willingness to cooperate with educational staff, integration into the group, problem behavior towards peers, prosocial behavior towards peers, play and task behavior, regulation of emotions) [40, 41], the Dortmund Developmental Screening for Kindergarten (DESK 3–6 R, 45 to 50 items depending on age, domains: fine motor skills, gross motor skills, social competence, social behavior, social interaction, attention and concentration, cognition and language, cognition, basic competence literacy, basic competence numeracy, language and communication) [41] or the questionnaire Competencies and Interests of Children (KOMPIK, 158 items across 11 domains: motor skills, social and emotional behavior, motivation, language and early literacy, maths, science, music, design, health, well-being, and social relationships) [42].
While these instruments meet scientific standards, they are all longer and quite time-consuming (minimum 40 items, while the DESK even contains performance tasks over and above questionnaire items, which requires even more time and a suitable physical environment in kindergartens). In addition, most of them do not feature SR as a separate construct and are far too comprehensive (e.g. measure development or behavioral issues in general), which reduces their suitability as efficient SR screening tools in the kindergarten environment and also might explain why they failed to gain wide use in Germany.
To move the field of developmental monitoring and public health intervention planning in kindergartens in Germany forward, we previously adapted the internationally widely used Canadian Early Development Instrument (EDI) [43] to the German context and published the German version of the EDI (GEDI) [44]. The EDI is a valid and reliable teacher 103-item questionnaire assessing a child’s ability to meet age-appropriate development expectations in five domains (see below), developed by Magdalena Janus and colleagues at the Offord Center for Child Studies at McMaster University, Ontario. The instrument was designed as a screening and developmental monitoring tool [4549]. It serves to collect data on the development of 3- to 6-year-old children in all relevant developmental domains [50]. In Canada and other countries, the EDI is integrated into a public health monitoring and intervention planning approach, which results in a tailored implementation of interventions in kindergartens to support child health and development.
Based on the features described above, the EDI could provide an optimal basis to develop a brief, but psychometrically sound and fully questionnaire-based screening instrument to detect SR difficulties in kindergarten children. In addition, the worldwide use of the EDI would allow to assess SR as part of the regular EDI monitoring in kindergartens in many countries.
Therefore, this study assesses whether it is possible to develop a valid scale measuring SR by recombining items of the theoretically relevant EDI domains "social competence" and "emotional maturity". The following research questions guide our study:
a)
Can existing items from the (G)EDI be selected based on solid theoretical and conceptual considerations and recombined to form a valid (stand-alone) SR scale?
 
b)
Does the resulting (G)EDI-SR scale have adequate psychometric properties and validity?
 

Methods

Recruitment, data collection and sample description

The present study collected data with the (G)EDI teacher questionnaire [43, 44] in two independent data sets – (a) the development dataset, with 191 children, collected in June 2016 to pilot the EDI in Germany in three different towns, with more details on recruitment and psychometric features published elsewhere [44], and b) the validation dataset, with 184 children, collected in fall 2021, in kindergartens in a small town in the South-West of Germany (population approx. 15.000), which intended to use the GEDI as the starting point for a community-based early childhood prevention strategy. In both data collections, teachers completed the full GEDI and the VSK-SR subscale for all participating children. The precondition to fill out the GEDI was that the teachers knew the children for at least one month, had sufficient command of the German language, and took part in a training session prior to the assessment. The previous training ensured that all teachers had the same level of knowledge about the instrument, its purpose and completion.
All data were collected electronically and given an individual pseudonym by the teachers to match first and second surveys to the same child with a 100% degree of accuracy.
Eligibility criteria for the children to whom the GEDI was administered comprised age 3 to 6 years, the presence of written informed parental consent and the absence of special needs. Table 1 displays descriptive characteristics for both samples and provides the number of eligible and finally participating children and teachers. Ethical approvals for both data collections were granted by the Ethics committee of the Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University (development sample: 2015-640N-MA; validation sample: 2016-588N-MA). The teachers’ participation was taken as an implicit consent to participate in our study.
Table 1
Characteristics of development and validation samples
  
N development sample (%)
N validation sample (%)
Eligible (invited)
Children
444
385
Kindergartens
9
6
Teachers
60
75
Participating
Children with parental consent
225 (51)
209 (54)
Kindergartens
9 (100)
6 (100)
Teachers
60 (100)
33(44)
 
Cases excluded upon reasons
34a (15)
25b (12)
 
Cases in dataset
191 (43)
184 (48)
 
mean age (range; SD)
4.27 (3 to 6; 1.05)
4.25 (3 to 6; 0.94)
 
n 3 years
58 (30)
46 (25)
 
n 4 years
60 (31)
65 (35)
 
n 5 years
43 (23)
55 (30)
 
n 6 years
30 (16)
18 (10)
 
Gender (female)
49%
51%
 
German second language
18%
7%
 
SES low/middle/high
2,6/49,2/40,3%
-
an = 5 with missing data or a “don’t know” response to the special needs assignation variable; n = 28 with special needs assignation, n = 1 under the age of three
bn = 22 due to an affirmative answer to the special needs question, n = 3 under the age of three
SES = socioeconomic status

Study design – overview

In a first step, the selection of GEDI items that theoretically map to SR was performed, which resulted in eligible GEDI-SR items. To assess the construct and dimensions of the eligible GEDI-SR items (see beneath), we used the development dataset, resulting in a first GEDI-SR scale. The GEDI data from the two independent samples were then used to cross-validate the item and factor structure of the GEDI-SR scale from the development data set to the validation data set. In a next step, using the validation data set, the GEDI-SR scale was compared with the VSK-SR items to assess concurrent validity of the GEDI-SR scale. Moreover, our reliability analyses used data from repeated retests of the GEDI within the validation sample. In the following, measurements and related statistical analyses for the different steps of the study design are presented in more detail.

Measurements

The GEDI as basis for SR scale development

The GEDI, like the original EDI, is a kindergarten teacher questionnaire to assess early childhood development in the following domains: “physical health and well-being" (13 items), "social competence" (26 items), "emotional maturity" (30 items), "language and cognitive development" (25 items), and "communication and general knowledge" (8 items) based on accumulated teacher impression and observation (and not on performance tasks). As a public health tool, the (G)EDI can be helpful in several ways: e.g. for teachers to create optimal learning opportunities tailored to individual child developmental profiles, for school boards and ministries to plan resource allocations to kindergartens (e.g. child-teacher relation) and to describe specific intervention needs in kindergartens which could be used for public health monitoring and planning (including to convince funders of intervention projects) [51].
The validation of the GEDI in the German context across the original five main domains demonstrated excellent internal consistency (0.73 < α > 0.99), moderate to good test–retest and interrater reliability (0.50 to 0.81 and 0.48 to 0.71, respectively [p-value < 0.05]), and good concurrent validity with other developmental instruments (range: 0.32 to 0.67) (details see [44]).
However, focus groups with teachers after the first data collection in Germany revealed a need to provide age-specific ratings (the original instrument is applied to 5-year old children in their preschool year, while in Germany kindergartens serve children from the age of 3 to 6). Using item response analyses, appropriateness of age-related information content and redundancies (e.g. some items from the original 103 items that did not provide additional content for specific age groups) were resolved, which thereby led to an overall shortening of the GEDI as compared to the EDI. The age-adjusted, age-specific and shorter GEDI contains different numbers of items, depending on the age group: n = 69 for 3–4 year-olds, n = 65 for 5-year-olds, and n = 61 for 6-year-olds. In the present study, only the items of the SR-relevant domains of the GEDI, "social competence" (n = 15 and 16 items for 3–4- as well as 5–6-year-olds, respectively) and "emotional maturity" (n = 21 items for all age groups), were considered and analysed.

The VSK as measure to assess concurrent validity

Besides the GEDI we applied the SR subscale of the German Kindergarten Behavioral Scales (Verhaltensskala für den Kindergarten = VSK-SR) [39] to assess concurrent validity. The VSK comprises 49 items in seven domains: anxiety, hyperactivity and inattention, aggressive behavior, emotional dysregulation, social competence, emotional knowledge/empathy, self-regulation). The VSK-SR scale entails five items, with an internal consistency of = 0.79: waits for his or her turn, performs activities he or she does not like, wants things immediately, considers the consequences of his or her own actions, finishes tasks. The concurrent validity of the VSK-SR subscale was assessed with the SDQ [35] and proved to be moderate (-0.67, p-value < 0.001) and thus acceptable [52].

Selection of items: Assessing eligibility and selecting SR-mapping GEDI items

We used a theory-based approach to identify items that might be relevant for the development of a SR scale. As a theoretical basis, we used a widely accepted categorization system of SR [4]. It considers SR as a multidimensional latent construct, including three closely related sub-dimensions: a) cognitive inhibition, which means the inhibition of thoughts and memories, b) selective or focused attention, or c) response inhibition: self-control/discipline. With these definitions in mind, three independent raters who were professionally familiar with early childhood development (childhood education, occupational therapy, developmental psychology) assessed all items within the GEDI domains of "social competence" and "emotional maturity”, which deemed relevant as these skills are closely related to SR skills [53]. Each item was labeled each as either 0 (not mapping to SR) or 1 (mapping to SR). Subsequently, they assigned the items mapping to SR to the three sub-dimensions of SR. Interrater agreement was assessed using kappa-statistics. Inconsistencies were resolved through discussion including a third independent rater until consensus was reached. This process resulted in items eligible to form the new GEDI-SR scale.

Statistical analyses

Operationalization and categorization of responses in the GEDI-SR scale
Like in the original EDI, we retained three-point Likert scales for the GEDI (coding: often/very true = 10, sometimes/somewhat true = 5, and never/not true = 0) [43]. Higher mean scores indicated better development. Children were excluded from analyses in a domain if ≥ 30% of values were missing [20]. In the absence of a normative German sample to establish valid cut-offs, and in line with the original EDI procedures, children who scored lower than the 10th percentile in the ensuing GEDI-SR scale were preliminarily deemed as “vulnerable” in terms of SR [54].
Descriptive analysis of the two data sets
We initially compared descriptive statistics of the development and validation datasets (sample size, mean age, distribution and scorings at 10th, 25ths, 50th and 75th percentile) using kernel density plots to reveal differences that might further help to explain potential inconsistencies in structured equation modeling (SEM).
Assessment of construct and dimensions of the eligible GEDI-SR items: Psychometric evaluation
We first performed an exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. Using the development dataset, we applied the measure of sampling adequacy (MSA, < 0.5 unsuitable, ≥ 0.6 usable, > 0.8 good [55]). To test the hypothesis regarding the factor structure among the eligible GEDI-SR items, we conducted an exploratory factor analysis using structural equation modeling (maximum-likelihood method). The comparative fit index (CFI, > 0.95) [56] and the root mean squared error of approximation (RMSEA, < 0.05) [5760] served as goodness-of-fit indicators of the model. To avoid overfitting, we tested the model fitted with the development dataset by recalculating the same model using the validation dataset. We aimed to replicate the main structured equation modeling composition of the model (confirmatory factor analysis). Since we were still in the exploration stage, we adjusted correlations among items in the validation dataset where necessary in favor of a better model fit.
Reliability testing of the GEDI-SR scale
We assessed internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) of the GEDI-SR scale resulting from the confirmatory factor analysis and used intraclass correlation coefficients (ICC) to assess test–retest and interrater reliability (0.5 = poor, 0.5 to 0.75 = moderate, 0.75 to 0.9 = good, and > 0.9 = excellent [61]. We asked teachers to repeat the GEDI for a randomly selected subset of children (n = 72; 3 children per age group) after two weeks. ICCs indicate the strength of the correlation of the GEDI-SR scores between the two measurement time points. The higher the ICC value, the better the correlation between T1 and T2 and the better the corresponding reliability. Additional plausibility checks using invariant demographic variables (birth quarter, gender) ensured the accuracy between T1 and T2 data.
Concurrent validity testing of the GEDI-SR scale
We assessed concurrent validity by means of Pearson correlation coefficients and plotting differences between the mean GEDI-SR and VSK-SR scores using Bland–Altman (BA) plots for each age group. BA plots are graphical representations that can be used to compare two measurement methods by analyzing the agreement between these: a difference plot combined with calculation of the two (upper and lower) limits of the differences between the methods (the so-called 95% limits of agreement). The x axis shows the mean of the results of the two methods ([A + B]/2), whereas the y axis represents the absolute difference between the two methods ([B—A]) [62, 63]. The closer the points in the plot are aligned around the line of mean difference (line centered at zero of the y-axis), the better the agreement. A good agreement is to be interpreted as good concurrent validity.
To meet the requirement for normality [64, 65], we used the Stata commands gladder and qladder and selected the closest to normal distribution. To enable cross-measure comparisons in BA plots, GEDI-SR and VSK-SR scores were transformed into z-scores. BA plots were generated using the Stata command concord [66]. The association between the two measures was examined by (i) considering the mean difference and (ii) the scattering of dots around the mean difference line in relation to the latent trait continuum on the x-axis.
All analyses were conducted using Stata (StataCorp. 2015. Stata Statistical Software: Release 15. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.).

Results

Results of the item selection process

The theory-based item selection resulted in a list of 20 eligible GEDI-SR items (Table 2). In the selection process, a moderate kappa of 0.5 between the three raters could be achieved.
Table 2
GEDI items to develop a self-regulation scale selected on a theoretical basis
Original GEDI-Domain
 
Items
Would you say that this child…
Social competence
qc2
Has the ability to get along with peers
qc5
Follows rules and instructions
qc7
Demonstrates self-control
qc9
Demonstrates respect for adults
qc10
Demonstrates respect for children
qc11
Accepts responsibility for actions
qc12
Listens attentively
qc14
Completes work on time
qc15
Works independently
qc16
Takes care of school materials
qc17
Works neatly and carefully
qc24
Is able to follow class routines without reminders
Emotional maturity
qc37
Gets into physical fights
qc42
Can’t sit still, is restless
qc43
Is distractible, has trouble sticking to any activity
qc44
Fidgets
qc46
Has temper tantrums
qc47
Is impulsive, acts without thinking
qc48
Has difficulty awaiting turn in games or groups
qc50
Is inattentive

Assessment of construct and dimensions of the eligible GEDI-SR items: Psychometric evaluation

The measure of sampling adequacy analysis amounted to MSA = 0.9. Exploratory factor analysis with the development sample revealed three highly significant (p-value < 0.001) interrelated factors (Table 3). The explanations in the right column of this table show that the loadings and allocations of the eligible items to the factors are theory-based and comprehensible. The contents of all items with loadings higher than or equal to 0.4 could be transparently assigned to the corresponding factors. Four items with loadings below 0.4 had too general a wording and their content did not necessarily refer to the ability to self-regulate. Therefore, they were removed from consideration leaving us with 16 of the initially 20 eligible items. Based on the theoretical background, the ensuing three factors were labeled as: 1) behavioral response inhibition; 2) cognitive inhibition; 3) selective or focused attention.
Table 3
Factor loadings and theory-based explanations resulting from exploratory factor analysis with the development dataset
Variable
Would you say that this child…
Factor1
Factor2
Factor3
Uniqueness
Theory based explanation
qc10
Demonstrates respect for children
0.7440
  
0.4707
Requires to inhibit emotions and behavior
qc9
Demonstrates respect for adults
0.6693
  
0.4933
Requires to inhibit emotions and behavior
qc37
Gets into physical fights
0.6625
  
0.6026
Requires to regulate emotions and needs a certain motivation to regulate behavior
qc47
Is impulsive, acts without thinking
0.6237
  
0.5022
Impulsivity is the inability to regulate emotions and behavior. If someone is planned, then he can regulate his emotions and act in a self-controlled manner
qc5
Follows rules and instructions
0.5344
  
0.6048
Requires the ability to motivate oneself to adapt and to inhibit "rebellious" emotions and behave accordingly
qc11
Accepts responsibility for actions
0.4134
  
0.4878
Requires the ability stand up for own mistakes to resist the impulse to be offended and "run away". This requires to regulate emotions and behavior by being honest and not offended
qc7a
Demonstrates self-control
 < 0.4
  
0.6445
Can mean anything and does not separate well. The item is not worded accurately enough
qc46a
Has temper tantrums
 < 0.4
  
0.8090
You can throw tantrums for very different reasons. However, this does not necessarily mean that one has a bad SR
qc15
Works independently
 
0.7598
 
0.4631
to be able to work independently, I have to be able to remember things and stay on task
qc17
Works neatly and carefully
 
0.7359
 
0.3816
to be neat and careful, I need to be able to structure myself and my thoughts
qc14
Completes work on time
 
0.7344
 
0.4662
To stay on schedule, I also need to be able to stay on task and focus my thoughts on what I'm doing
qc24
Is able to follow class routines without reminders
 
0.5504
 
0.6140
Requires the ability to remember things and also be able to recall it again
qc12
Listens attentively
 
0.5315
 
0.4751
Requires the ability to block out disturbing thoughts and memories
qc16
Takes care of school materials
 
0.5002
 
0.4667
Requires to be careful and not destroy anything on purpose. Requires the ability to suppress the impulse to destroy, which is sometimes perceptible, and behave appropriately and in a controlled manner
qc2a
Has the ability to get along with peers
 
 < 0.4
 
0.7658
Too many things in one item. Doesn't have to be SR ability if someone can get along with another kid
qc44
Fidgets
  
0.7379
0.4575
Fidgeting and being restless and physically active doesn't necessarily mean that a child is not able to concentrate, but certainly often goes hand in hand with it
qc43
Is distractible, has trouble sticking to any activity
  
0.7372
0.4048
Requires the ability to concentrate and focus attention
qc42
Can’t sit still, is restless
  
0.7171
0.4387
These children have difficulties to focus their attention
qc50
Is inattentive
  
0.6705
0.4803
These children can't concentrate and selectively focus their attention
qc48a
Has difficulty awaiting turn in games or groups
  
 < 0.4
0.6657
Awaiting turn requires patience and waiting is different from attention and concentration
Note: aItem excluded from subsequent analysis (structured equation modeling and BA-Plots)
Item numbers in bold: Items corresponding with VSK-SR items: Waits for his or her turn (qc5), Performs activities he or she does not like (qc11), Wants things immediately (qc48), Considers the consequences of his or her own actions (qc11, qc47), Finishes tasks (qc14, qc43)
Confirmatory factor analysis with the development dataset using structured equation modeling revealed highly significant correlations at the factor and item level. Three items loaded below 0.6 and were therefore excluded from the final model (Table 4) leaving us with 13 items of the initially 20 eligible items. The good model fit (RMSEA: 0.029, CFI: 0.993) is presented in Table 4, resulting in a 13-item SR scale to be tested further.
Table 4
Factor structure, Item- and Subdomain correlations of the latent construct self-regulation using structured equation modeling
   
development dataset
replication with validation dataset
   
N = 191
N = 184
Factor
Item
 
Coefficient
(subdomain level)
Coefficient
(item level)
SE
95% CI lb
95% CI ub
Correlations with other items
Coefficient
(subdomain level)
Coefficient
(item-level)
SE
95% CI lb
95% CI ub
Correlations with other items
sd16 discipl
qc10
Demonstrates respect for children
sd17 0.76***
sd18 0.62***
0.64***
0.05
0.54
0.75
qc9 0.26**
qc15 -0.14 (ns)
qc14 -0.19*
qc16 0.13 (ns)
sd17 0.66***
sd18 0.77***
0.56***
0.06
0.45
0.68
qc9 0.52***
qc16 0.39***
qc9
Demonstrates respect for adults
0.67***
0.05
0.56
0.77
 
0.49***
0.06
0.37
0.61
qc16 0.29***
qc42 0.19***
qc50 -0.15*
qc37
Gets into physical fights
 
 < 0.6
excluded
      
qc47
Is impulsive, acts without thinking
 
 < 0.6
excluded
      
qc5
Follows rules and instructions
0.68***
0.05
0.58
0.78
qc15 -0.19*
0.97***
0.06
0.86
1.08
qc11 -1.67(ns)
qc16 0.58(ns)
q11
Accepts responsibility for actions
0.72***
0.05
0.63
0.81
 
0.86***
0.06
0.74
0.98
qc15 0.43***
qc17 0.38**
qc16 0.63***
sd17
cog
qc15
Works independently
sd18 0.57***
0.5
0.05
0.49
0.71
qc17 0.13 (ns)
qc14 0.225**
qc44 -0.15*
sd18 0.89***
0.69***
0.05
0.59
0.79
qc17 0.23**
qc14 0.13(ns)
qc12 -0.28*
qc42 -0.35***
qc17
Works neatly and carefully
0.6***
0.04
0.76
0.90
qc12 -0.35**
0.71***
0.05
0.62
0.80
qc12 -0.49***
qc42 -0.18*
qc50 0.14(ns)
qc14
Completes work on time
0.68***
0.05
0.59
0.77
qc16 -0.19*
0.69***
0.04
0.61
0.77
 
qc24
Is able to follow class routines without reminders
 
 < 0.6
excluded
      
qc12
Listens attentively
0.74***
0.04
0.65
0.82
qc43 0.28**
0.86***
0.03
0.81
0.92
 
qc16
Takes care of school materials
0.76***
0.04
0.68
0.83
 
0.67***
0.04
0.58
0.75
qc42 -0.14*
sd18
att
qc44
Fidgets
 
0.64***
0.05
0.54
0.75
qc42 0.29***
 
0.73***
0.04
0.65
0.81
qc42 0.45***
qc43
Is distractible, has trouble sticking to any activity
0.81***
0.04
0.73
0.89
 
0.87***
0.02
0.82
0.92
 
qc42
Can't sit still, is restless
0.71***
0.05
0.61
0.80
 
0.76***
0.04
0.69
0.83
 
qc50
Is inattentive
0.71***
0.05
0.62
0.80
 
0.77***
0.03
0.70
0.84
 
RMSEA /CFI
 
0.029 / 0.993
0.019 / 0.998
sd  Subdomain, SE Standard error, CI Confidence interval, RMSEA Root mean square error of approximation, CFI Comparative fit index
sd16 = Behavioral response inhibition
sd17 = Cognitive inhibition
sd18 = Selective or focused attention
Note: *p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, ***p = 0.000

Cross-validation: confirmatory analysis using the validation dataset

We tried to replicate the GEDI-SR scale model using the validation dataset. This cross-validation yielded similar results (RMSEA: 0.019, CFI: 0.998) (Table 4.), confirming the 13-item scale within a three-factor model structure.

Comparison of the 13-item GEDI-SR scale’s descriptive data across the datasets

Overall, descriptive statistics and age-specific kernel density plots for development and validation samples (Table 5, Fig. 1) illustrate the underlying distribution of the data. The mean value of the 10% cut-off in the samples ranged from 5.00 in the development data set to 5.42 in the validation data set, respectively. The graph shows the similarly skewed distribution in both datasets except for 3- and 4-year old children, whose percentile values partially differ from each other up to 1.4 points.
Table 5
Descriptive statistics for both the development and validation datasets
Participant information
GEDI-SR scale scores
Age
N
Mean
SD
Min
Max
10th
25th
75th
development sample
 3
58
7.60
1.62
3.08
10
5.00
6.92
8.85
 4
60
7.52
2.02
3.08
10
4.42
6.54
9.23
 5
43
8.28
1.53
4.62
10
5.77
7.31
9.62
 6
30
8.68
1.60
2.69
10
6.92
8.08
9.62
overall
191
7.90
1.78
2.69
10
5.00
6.92
9.23
validation sample
 3
46
7.26
2.31
2.08
10
3.75
5.83
9.58
 4
65
8.26
1.84
2.08
10
5.83
7.50
9.58
 5
55
8.44
1.98
2.08
10
5.42
7.50
10
 6
18
8.96
1.36
5.42
10
7.50
7.92
10
overall
184
8.13
2.03
2.08
10
5.42
7.08
10

Internal consistency, test–retest and interrater reliability results

Internal consistency (range: 0.89 < ⍺ > 0.92), overall test–retest ICC (0.85, 95%-CI: 0.71 to 0.93), and overall interrater ICC (0.71, 95%-CI: 0.43 to 0.89) of the 13-item GEDI-SR scale were good (Table 6). For test–retest and interrater reliability we obtained 27 (38%) retest pairs and 26 (36%) interrater pairs (children at least 3 years old, without special needs). The interval between T1 and T2 ranged from 6 and 9 to 30 and 22 days, respectively. Attempting to balance between "include as many pairs as possible" and "the interval between T1 and T2 should be as close to 14 days as possible" we only included pairs with a time interval between 13 and 15 days (n = 25 and 17 pairs). Due to a large score difference between T1 and T2 in some pairs, retest ICCs could not be calculated for 6-year-olds and interrater ICCs could only be calculated for 3-year-olds. Therefore, we only report the overall ICCs in Table 6.
Table 6
Reliability of the GEDI-SR scale
 
age
N
Cronbach's alpha
Internal consistency
3 y
46
0.92
4 y
65
0.9
5 y
55
0.92
6 y
18
0.89
overall
184
0.92
  
N (pairs)
ICCs (CI)
Test–retest reliability
across age groups
25
0.85 (0.71 to 0.93)
Interrater reliability
17
0.71 (0.43 to 0.89)

Concurrent validity

Table 7 shows the results from assessing concurrent validity. With one exception, correlation coefficients indicate strong, statistically significant positive linear correlations in all age groups (range: 0.61 to 0.84). Limits of agreement are furthest apart for 6-year-olds and closest for 5-year-olds (Table 8.). Figures 2 A to E illustrate the extent to which the paired variables match. The more dispersed scatter of points around the mid-section in Figures A, B, C, and E reveal that the poorest agreement is for children with average SR skills. Children with lower average SR skills (scores <  − 1 on the x-axis) and those with higher average SR skills (scores > 1 on the x-axis) tend to be underestimated with the GEDI-SR scale compared to the VSK-SR scale. In plot D (5-year-olds), dots are clustered more tightly around the line of mean difference in the mid-section of the x-axis, indicating good agreement between the GEDI-SR and VSK-SR scales in the latent trait section, where the vast majority of children scored. For children with extreme values around -3, the plot shows a larger measurement error to the extent that the GEDI-SR scale underestimates children in the lower latent trait range.
Table 7
Pearson correlation between the GEDI-SR scale and the VSK-SR scale
  
GEDI-SR scale
 
  
3 years
4 years
5 years
6 years
Overall
VSK subdomain SR
3 years
0.72***
    
4 years
 
0.70***
   
5 years
  
0.84***
  
6 years
   
0.61**
 
overall
    
0.75***
Note: VSK „Kindergarten Behavioral Scales “, GEDI  German version of the Early development instrument, SR Self-regulation, *** p = 0.000
Table 8
Concurrent validity: Mean differences between GEDI-SR scale and VSK-SR scale
Age-group
N
Difference Average /
Mean difference
SD
95%-Limits of agreement
Concordance correlation coefficient
Pearson's r (95%-CI)
3 years
46
-0.000a
0.75
-1.47 to 1.47
0.72*** (0.54 to 0.83)
4 years
65
-0.000a
0.77
-1.52 to 1.52
0,70*** (0.55 to 0.81)
5 years
55
0.000
0.56
-1.11 to 1.11
0,84*** (0.74 to 0.90)
6 years
18
-0.000a
0.92
-1.8 to 1.8
0.58** (0.17 to 0.82)
overall
184
0.000
0.71
-1.38 to 1.38
0.75*** (0.68 to 0.81)
Note: SD Standard deviation, CI Confidence interval, *** = p < 0.001; a values have a slightly negative tendency, which only becomes apparent after the fourth comma position

Discussion

The aim of the study was to identify items eligible for SR-measurement within the (G)EDI domains "social competence" and "emotional maturity" by a theory-based selection process, and therefrom develop a GEDI-SR scale and assess its dimensions, psychometric properties and validity.
We identified 20 original (G)EDI items eligible for measuring SR. Starting with these 20 items, we used exploratory factor analysis to assess constructs and dimensions using the development dataset. Cross-validation with both datasets using confirmatory factor analysis was successful and resulted in a 13-item, three-factor GEDI-SR scale model with excellent goodness of fit indices for measuring SR in kindergarten children. The GEDI-SR scale’s internal consistency, test–retest and interrater reliability, stability across populations as well as concurrent validity with the VSK-SR scale were in the good to excellent range, which qualifies the scale for screening or monitoring purposes. Since all items of this SR scale are inherent to the (G)EDI, SR can now be efficiently measured when administering the (G)EDI, without the need for applying an additional SR assessment instrument. Alternatively, given high reliability and validity, the newly developed, short GEDI-SR scale could also be administered as stand-alone scale.

Development of the GEDI-SR scale and its constructs and dimensions

The sequence of theory-based selection process and a subsequent quantitative analysis of constructs and dimensions of the resulting eligible SR-items across two independent data sets was successful to reduce the initial 20 items to a very short scale of 13 items to measure SR in a valid way. The internal consistency of this scale was high (⍺ 〜 0.90).
The 13 items of the resulting SR scale revealed large correlations at the factor and item level, which indicates a multicomponent latent construct. The three factors of the GEDI-SR scale found empirically correspond perfectly to the theoretical basis of Diamond's conceptual model on SR [33], which underlines the scale’s validity. It consists of the “core” components of SR 1) behavioral response inhibition; 2) cognitive inhibition; 3) selective or focused attention (Diamond 2013). A child scoring high on these domains will find it easier to a) meet teachers' expectations, as teachers expect children to behave appropriately with regard to their school readiness and show SR by treating people and things well, by being able to sit still and to listen when needed [67]. Such children will show b) responsible behavior by following rules, taking responsibility for their actions, and being mindful of the materials and furniture at the kindergarten; c) concentration being able to conduct activities independently and calmly, e.g. completing painting and handicrafts carefully and on time, and to have an appropriate attention span. Children with high levels of SR may be expected to show d) conscientiousness, for example being careful with play materials.
The exploratory factor analysis led to omission of four items from the eligible SR-item selection. These encompass items such as “demonstrates self-control”, “has temper tantrums”, “has the ability to get along with peers” and “has difficulty awaiting turn in games or groups”, which -based on face-validity- might actually relate to the concept of SR. It is therefore not fully clear why the exploratory factor analysis suggested omission. The most probably hypothesis is that these items capture other behavioral domains distinct from the 13-items representing SR. Likewise, the structural equation modeling failed to support the inclusion of the items “gets into physical fights”, “is impulsive, acts without thinking” and “is able to follow class routines without reminders” – although all three investigators initially considered them to be appropriate and relevant items to measure SR. This however does not seem unusual: Also other studies on the development of theory- or literature-based questionnaires have shown that theoretically relevant items are dropped after factor analytic steps [68, 69]. Authors have argued that this might be due to the wording of some items not being appropriate to reflect the latent construct for which they were actually included.

Reliability assessment

The 13-item GEDI-SR scale showed favorable reliability, both with respect to internal consistency as well as the results from structural equation modeling and re-test analyses. Yet, we must acknowledge some limitations regarding test–retest and interrater reliability. First, due to the COVID-19 pandemic and difficult organizational conditions in kindergartens, we received significantly fewer pairs of data than intended. With three pairs only for 6-year-olds, calculation of ICCs was not possible as was the calculation of interrater ICCs for 4- to 6-year-olds. We therefore only present overall values and recommend age-specific reliability analysis in a future study.

Concurrent validity

We assessed concurrent validity by comparison to the VSK-SR scale. The VSK-SR scale tends to focus behavioral inhibition, namely patience, adaptability, and perseverance skills, whereas the GEDI-SR scale reflects cognitive inhibition and selective/focused attention with slightly different dimensions (concentration, diligence, and adherence to rules). Given this difference, the degree of agreement in terms of Pearson’s correlation coefficient was good. However, despite good overall concurrent validity results, the additional Bland–Altman analysis revealed that the two scales ((G)EDI-SR versus VSK-SR) differed for extreme values of SR. It thus remains uncertain whether the VSK-SR overestimates the extremes or the GEDI-SR underestimates deviations from the mean. Therefore, a future study might want to re-investigate the agreement of the GEDI-SR scale and another instrument available in German language, such as the SDQ.

Comparison of reliability and validity results with those of other SR instruments

Regarding its psychometric properties and validity, the GEDI-SR scale shows values comparable (or even superior) to those of other instruments used to measure SR in the international and national context, as exemplified and quantified in Table 9. For example, the GEDI-SR scale compared to the other instruments shows very good internal consistency. Test–retest reliability seems even better than that of the CBQ or SDQ.
Table 9
Comparison of psychometric properties of the GEDI-SR scale with other SR-measurements
 
GEDI-SR
Other SR-Measurements
  
CBCL 1,5–5
CBQa
SDQ
BRIEF-P Canadian Sample
BRIEF-P German Sample
Psychometric properties
Current study
Achenbach (2000)
Putnam (2006)
Goodman (2001)
Shermand (2010)
Daseking (2013)
Reliability
Internal consistency
⍺ 〜 0.90
⍺ ≥ 0.86
⍺ = 0.67 to 0.71
⍺ = 0.73
0.90 < ⍺ > 0.97
0.82 < ⍺ > 0.94
Test–retest reliability
ICC = 0.85 (95%-CI: 0.71 to 0.93)
Pearson’s r = 0.72 to 0.89
r = 0.61 – 0.70
r = 0.73 (after 4 to 6 months)
r =  ≥ 0.90
X
Interrater reliability
ICC: 0.71 (95%-CI: 0.43 to 0.89)
r = 0.52 to 0.78
r = 0.47
r = 0.80; (sample of 5–15-Year-Olds)
X
r = 0.56
Validity
Concurrent Validity
r = 0.75 with VSK-SR
r = 0.56 to 0.77 with the Richman Behavior Checklist
X
OR = 13.5 (95%-CI: 11.1 to 16.3) with DSM-IV-diagnosis
X
r = 0.70 with BASCb
Note: aValues for parents as respondents; bBASC Behavioral Assessment of Children (Reynolds & Kamphaus 2004), X Information not available, ICC Intraclass correlation coefficient
Moreover, our results confirm the good psychometric properties of the original (G)EDI and show that the "Social Competence" and "Emotional Maturity" scales of the EDI have been developed very well with regard to the selection and formulation of items. Building on this excellent work of the Canadian developers, we were now able to develop a reliable and valid SR scale that is inherent to the (G)EDI and thus does not require additional time for SR-assessment.

Public health implication

Given good psychometric characteristics, high validity and reliability of the (G)EDI-SR scale, our work is the precondition for a public health monitoring process, which could take GEDI-SR as part of the (G)EDI or as a stand-alone scale as a starting point for intervention implementation, both at the individual child as well as the population level. The newly developed GEDI-SR might be specifically relevant to those countries already monitoring child development in kindergartens using the EDI at scale (e.g., Australia [45]). However, to lever its use as a potential public health screening instrument, in a next step, age-specific standardized cut-offs should be established in a representative sample (standardization sample) [70]. After the establishment of valid cut-off values, each country using the EDI for developmental monitoring could efficiently screen for SR difficulties in this early age and use the screening for tailored implementation of SR-promoting interventions in kindergartens at a public health scale.

Strengths and limitations

To our best knowledge, this is the first study to define and validate a short SR scale within the widely used EDI. Although other short SR subscales exist (e.g. in the VSK-SR or the CBRS) and might be theoretically usable, our scale might be very efficient from a public health perspective as its items are part of and included in the administration of the EDI or GEDI. In addition, the costly purchase of e.g. the VSK (which is not open access) and the necessary, separate scoring methodology make the use of a separate SR scale potentially challenging for teachers and public health researchers, especially if compared to the (G)EDI assessment, which would allow developmental and SR assessment at once and is available free of charge.
In terms of item selection for the GEDI-SR scale, we only achieved a moderate agreement between raters, which underscores the difficulty to distinguish SR from other constructs such as social competence or emotional maturity. Despite the agreement and consensus regarding the theoretical basis, the only moderate agreement might also be explained by the raters’ different professional perspective and background (psychology, occupational therapy, pedagogy), e.g. bringing about different preferences for wordings and deviating operationalizations. However, reassuringly, the results of our exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses and structured equation modeling suggest that the selected items represent the latent construct SR.
Although we were able to include two independent data sets, we are aware that both might be affected by selection bias, according to their geographic location (e.g. potentially containing lower numbers of children from families with low socioeconomic status). As we did not collect the SES of the children's families we cannot assess representativeness of the samples. Hence, our data cannot readily be generalized to specific subgroups of interest, for example children from parents with recent migrant background and lower socio-economic or educational status. Moreover, 6-year-old children are underrepresented in both datasets. We found differing percentile values for lower age groups, but we attribute these to a higher inter- and intra-individual variability of developmental maturity [71].
In addition, we did not establish reference values in a representative data set. However, given the successful replication of the structured equation modeling with the validation dataset, we were at last able to demonstrate the stability of the model across populations. Last, at this stage and without a standardized sample, we are currently unable to determine the predictive validity of the GEDI-SR scale.

Conclusion

Thirteen items in the (G)EDI can be recombined to a reliable and valid (G)EDI-SR scale, which can be used either as a stand-alone scale or as part of regular developmental monitoring using the EDI or GEDI in kindergartens. Through using the SR scale as part of (G)EDI kindergarten monitoring, kindergartens with higher percentages of children with SR difficulties could be identified and interventions implemented in a tailored way. Future research collecting data with the GEDI-SR in a representative sample could provide appropriate age- and domain-specific standardized cut-offs that would enable an adequate evaluation of area-wide population-based data.

Acknowledgements

We thank the children, their parents and families, the kindergartens and regular kindergarten teachers for their cooperation (donated time as well as kindergarten teachers who participated in the expert interviews). Moreover, we thank the health manager of the community where data were collected, Franziska Kramer-Gmeiner, for her extraordinary support in recruitment and project coordination.

Declarations

Ethical approval was granted by the Ethics committee of the Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University (2015-640N-MA). The teachers’ participation was understood as an implicit consent to participate in our study. Written informed consent was obtained from parents. We confirm that all methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.
Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, et al. A Gradient of Childhood Self-Control Predicts Health, Wealth, and Public Safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011;108:2693–8.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky D, et al. A Gradient of Childhood Self-Control Predicts Health, Wealth, and Public Safety. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2011;108:2693–8.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Robson DA, Allen MS, Howard SJ. Self-regulation in childhood as a predictor of future outcomes: A meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. 2020;146:324–54.PubMedCrossRef Robson DA, Allen MS, Howard SJ. Self-regulation in childhood as a predictor of future outcomes: A meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. 2020;146:324–54.PubMedCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Gawrilow C, Rauch W. Selbstregulationsfähigkeiten und exekutive Funktionen im Entwicklungsverlauf bei Vorschulkindern und Schulkindern. (Self-regulatory and executive functions over the developmental course in preschoolers and school-age children.). In: Hartmann U, Gold A, Marcus H (eds) Entwicklungsverläufe verstehen - Kinder mit Bildungsrisiken wirksam fördern - Forschungsergebnisse des Frankfurter IDeA-Zentrums. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer; 2017. Gawrilow C, Rauch W. Selbstregulationsfähigkeiten und exekutive Funktionen im Entwicklungsverlauf bei Vorschulkindern und Schulkindern. (Self-regulatory and executive functions over the developmental course in preschoolers and school-age children.). In: Hartmann U, Gold A, Marcus H (eds) Entwicklungsverläufe verstehen - Kinder mit Bildungsrisiken wirksam fördern - Forschungsergebnisse des Frankfurter IDeA-Zentrums. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer; 2017.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Calkins SD, Graziano PA, Keane SP. Cardiac vagal regulation differentiates among children at risk for behavior problems. Biol Psychol. 2007;74:144–53.PubMedCrossRef Calkins SD, Graziano PA, Keane SP. Cardiac vagal regulation differentiates among children at risk for behavior problems. Biol Psychol. 2007;74:144–53.PubMedCrossRef
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Spinrad TL, et al. Longitudinal Relations of Children’s Effortful Control, Impulsivity, and Negative Emotionality to Their Externalizing, Internalizing, and Co-Occurring Behavior Problems. Dev Psychol. 2009;45:988–1008.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Eisenberg N, Valiente C, Spinrad TL, et al. Longitudinal Relations of Children’s Effortful Control, Impulsivity, and Negative Emotionality to Their Externalizing, Internalizing, and Co-Occurring Behavior Problems. Dev Psychol. 2009;45:988–1008.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Nigg JT. Annual Research Review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58:361–83.PubMedCrossRef Nigg JT. Annual Research Review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;58:361–83.PubMedCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Riggs NR, Kobayakawa Sakuma K-L, Pentz MA. Preventing Risk for Obesity by Promoting Self-Regulation and Decision-Making Skills. Pilot Results From the PATHWAYS to Health Program (PATHWAYS). Eval Rev. 2011;11:287–310. Riggs NR, Kobayakawa Sakuma K-L, Pentz MA. Preventing Risk for Obesity by Promoting Self-Regulation and Decision-Making Skills. Pilot Results From the PATHWAYS to Health Program (PATHWAYS). Eval Rev. 2011;11:287–310.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat White BA, Jarrett MA, Ollendick TH. Self-regulation deficits explain the link between reactive aggression and internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in children. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2013;35:1–9.CrossRef White BA, Jarrett MA, Ollendick TH. Self-regulation deficits explain the link between reactive aggression and internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in children. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 2013;35:1–9.CrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Hölling H, Erhart M, Ravens-Sieberer U, et al. Verhaltensauffälligkeiten bei Kindern und Jugendlichen: Erste Ergebnisse aus dem Kinder- und Jugendgesundheitssurvey (KiGGS) (Behavioral problems in children and adolescents: Initial findings from the Child and Adolescent Health Survey.). Bundesgesundheitsblatt Gesundheitsforschung Gesundheitsschutz. 2007;50:784–93.PubMedCrossRef Hölling H, Erhart M, Ravens-Sieberer U, et al. Verhaltensauffälligkeiten bei Kindern und Jugendlichen: Erste Ergebnisse aus dem Kinder- und Jugendgesundheitssurvey (KiGGS) (Behavioral problems in children and adolescents: Initial findings from the Child and Adolescent Health Survey.). Bundesgesundheitsblatt Gesundheitsforschung Gesundheitsschutz. 2007;50:784–93.PubMedCrossRef
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Klipker K, Baumtarten F, Göbel K, et al. Mental Health Problems in Children and Adolescents in Germany. Results of the Cross-Sectional KiGGS Wave 2 Study and Trends. J Health Monit. 2018;3:34–41.PubMedPubMedCentral Klipker K, Baumtarten F, Göbel K, et al. Mental Health Problems in Children and Adolescents in Germany. Results of the Cross-Sectional KiGGS Wave 2 Study and Trends. J Health Monit. 2018;3:34–41.PubMedPubMedCentral
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Nakamura YM, Lehmann RJ. Mitarbeiter/Innenbeurteilung. Lebens- und Schulqualität PH Akzente. 2003;3:50–2. Nakamura YM, Lehmann RJ. Mitarbeiter/Innenbeurteilung. Lebens- und Schulqualität PH Akzente. 2003;3:50–2.
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Nodi M, Ackermann K, Eberhard U, et al. Arbeitsbedingungen, Belastungen und Ressourcen von Lehrpersonen und Schulleitungen im Kanton Aargau 2008. Ergebnisse der Untersuchung im Auftrag des Departements Bildung, Kultur und Sport. (Working Conditions, Burdens and Resources of Teachers and School Administrators in the Canton of Aargau 2008. Aarau: Results of the Study Commissioned by the Department of Education, Culture and Sport.); 2008. Nodi M, Ackermann K, Eberhard U, et al. Arbeitsbedingungen, Belastungen und Ressourcen von Lehrpersonen und Schulleitungen im Kanton Aargau 2008. Ergebnisse der Untersuchung im Auftrag des Departements Bildung, Kultur und Sport. (Working Conditions, Burdens and Resources of Teachers and School Administrators in the Canton of Aargau 2008. Aarau: Results of the Study Commissioned by the Department of Education, Culture and Sport.); 2008.
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Keller R, Kunz A, Luder R, et al. Schulentwicklung für eine inklusive und gesunde Schule am Beispiel der Projekte „SIS“ und „Challenge“. (School development for an inclusive and healthy school using the example of the ‘SIS’ and ‘Challenge’ projects.). In: Zala-Mezö E, Strauss N-C, Häbig J, editors., et al., Dimensionen von Schulentwicklung. Verständnis, Veränderung und Vielfalt eines Phänomens. Münster. Münster: Waxmann; 2018. p. 187–204. Keller R, Kunz A, Luder R, et al. Schulentwicklung für eine inklusive und gesunde Schule am Beispiel der Projekte „SIS“ und „Challenge“. (School development for an inclusive and healthy school using the example of the ‘SIS’ and ‘Challenge’ projects.). In: Zala-Mezö E, Strauss N-C, Häbig J, editors., et al., Dimensionen von Schulentwicklung. Verständnis, Veränderung und Vielfalt eines Phänomens. Münster. Münster: Waxmann; 2018. p. 187–204.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Campbell SB, Pierce EW, March CL, et al. Hard-to-Manage Preschool Boys : Symptomatic Behavior across Contexts and Time. Child Dev. 1994;65:836–51.PubMedCrossRef Campbell SB, Pierce EW, March CL, et al. Hard-to-Manage Preschool Boys : Symptomatic Behavior across Contexts and Time. Child Dev. 1994;65:836–51.PubMedCrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Klora M, Zeidler J, Linder R, et al. Costs and treatment patterns of incident ADHD patients - a comparative analysis before and after the initial diagnosis -. Health Econ Rev. 2015;5:1–9.CrossRef Klora M, Zeidler J, Linder R, et al. Costs and treatment patterns of incident ADHD patients - a comparative analysis before and after the initial diagnosis -. Health Econ Rev. 2015;5:1–9.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Ewest F, Reinhold T, Vloet TD, et al. Durch Jugendliche mit Störungen des Sozialverhaltens ausgelöste Krankenkassenausgaben: Eine gesundheitsökonomische Analyse von Versichertendaten einer gesetzlichen Krankenkasse. (Health insurance costs caused by adolescents with social behavior disorders: A Health Economic Analysis of Insured Data from a Public Health Insurance Fund.). Kindheit und Entwicklung. 2013;22:41–7.CrossRef Ewest F, Reinhold T, Vloet TD, et al. Durch Jugendliche mit Störungen des Sozialverhaltens ausgelöste Krankenkassenausgaben: Eine gesundheitsökonomische Analyse von Versichertendaten einer gesetzlichen Krankenkasse. (Health insurance costs caused by adolescents with social behavior disorders: A Health Economic Analysis of Insured Data from a Public Health Insurance Fund.). Kindheit und Entwicklung. 2013;22:41–7.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Blair C. How similar are fluid cognition and general intelligence? A developmental neuroscience perspective on fluid cognition as an aspect of human cognitive ability. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2006;29:109–25.PubMedCrossRef Blair C. How similar are fluid cognition and general intelligence? A developmental neuroscience perspective on fluid cognition as an aspect of human cognitive ability. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2006;29:109–25.PubMedCrossRef
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Ceci SJ. How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence. Dev Psychol. 1991;27:703–22.CrossRef Ceci SJ. How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence. Dev Psychol. 1991;27:703–22.CrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Cicchetti D. The impact of social experience on neurobiological systems: illustration from a constructivist view of child maltreatment. Cogn Dev. 2002;17:1407–28.CrossRef Cicchetti D. The impact of social experience on neurobiological systems: illustration from a constructivist view of child maltreatment. Cogn Dev. 2002;17:1407–28.CrossRef
22.
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Pandey A, Hale D, Das S, et al. Effectiveness of universal self-regulation-based interventions in children and adolescents a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2018;172:566–75.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Pandey A, Hale D, Das S, et al. Effectiveness of universal self-regulation-based interventions in children and adolescents a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2018;172:566–75.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Muir RA, Howard SJ, Kervin L. Interventions and Approaches Targeting Early Self-Regulation or Executive Functioning in Preschools: A Systematic Review. Educ Psychol Rev. 2023;35:27.CrossRef Muir RA, Howard SJ, Kervin L. Interventions and Approaches Targeting Early Self-Regulation or Executive Functioning in Preschools: A Systematic Review. Educ Psychol Rev. 2023;35:27.CrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Epstein MH, Sharma JM. Behavioral and Emotional Rating Scale: A strength-based approach to Assessment. TX: PRO-ED; 1998. Epstein MH, Sharma JM. Behavioral and Emotional Rating Scale: A strength-based approach to Assessment. TX: PRO-ED; 1998.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Achenbach TM. The Child Behavior Checklist and related instruments. In: Maruish ME, editor. The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcomes assessment. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 1999. p. 429–66. Achenbach TM. The Child Behavior Checklist and related instruments. In: Maruish ME, editor. The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcomes assessment. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 1999. p. 429–66.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Putnam SP, Rothbart MK. Development of Short and Very Short Forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. J Pers Assess. 2006;87:102–12.PubMedCrossRef Putnam SP, Rothbart MK. Development of Short and Very Short Forms of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. J Pers Assess. 2006;87:102–12.PubMedCrossRef
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Bronson MB, Goodson BD, Layzer JI, et al. Child behavior rating scale. Cambridge MA: Abt Associates; 1990. Bronson MB, Goodson BD, Layzer JI, et al. Child behavior rating scale. Cambridge MA: Abt Associates; 1990.
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Conners CK. A teacher rating scale for use in drug studies with children. American J Psychiatry. 1969;126(6):884–8.CrossRef Conners CK. A teacher rating scale for use in drug studies with children. American J Psychiatry. 1969;126(6):884–8.CrossRef
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Lebuffe PA, Naglieri JA. The Devereux Early Childhood Assessment (for children ages 2 through 5 years). 1998. Lebuffe PA, Naglieri JA. The Devereux Early Childhood Assessment (for children ages 2 through 5 years). 1998.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Lafreniere PJ, Dumas JE. Social Competence and Behavior Evaluation in Children Ages 3 to 6 Years: The Short Form (SCBE-30). Psychol Assess. 1996;8:369–77.CrossRef Lafreniere PJ, Dumas JE. Social Competence and Behavior Evaluation in Children Ages 3 to 6 Years: The Short Form (SCBE-30). Psychol Assess. 1996;8:369–77.CrossRef
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Gouley KK, Brotman LM, Huang KY, et al. Construct validation of the social competence scale in preschool-age children. Soc Dev. 2008;17:380–98.CrossRef Gouley KK, Brotman LM, Huang KY, et al. Construct validation of the social competence scale in preschool-age children. Soc Dev. 2008;17:380–98.CrossRef
34.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–6.PubMedCrossRef Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:581–6.PubMedCrossRef
35.
Zurück zum Zitat Goodman R. Psychometric Properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:1337–45.PubMedCrossRef Goodman R. Psychometric Properties of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40:1337–45.PubMedCrossRef
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, et al. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Child Neuropsychol. 2000;6:235–8.CrossRef Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, et al. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Child Neuropsychol. 2000;6:235–8.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat McCoy DC. Measuring Young Children’s Executive Function and Self-Regulation in Classrooms and Other Real-World Settings. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2019;22:63–74.PubMedCrossRef McCoy DC. Measuring Young Children’s Executive Function and Self-Regulation in Classrooms and Other Real-World Settings. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2019;22:63–74.PubMedCrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Daseking M, Petermann F. Verhaltensinventar zur Beurteilung exekutiver Funktionen für das Kindergartenalter Deutschsprachige Adaptation des Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function® - Preschool Version (BRIEF®-P) von Gerard A. Gioia, Kimberly Andrews Espy und Peter K. Isquith. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2013. Daseking M, Petermann F. Verhaltensinventar zur Beurteilung exekutiver Funktionen für das Kindergartenalter Deutschsprachige Adaptation des Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function® - Preschool Version (BRIEF®-P) von Gerard A. Gioia, Kimberly Andrews Espy und Peter K. Isquith. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2013.
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Koglin U, Petermann F. Verhaltensskalen für das Kindergartenalter. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2016. Koglin U, Petermann F. Verhaltensskalen für das Kindergartenalter. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2016.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Seeger D, Holodynski M, Souvignier E. Testbesprechung. BIKO-Screening zur Entwicklung von Basiskompetenzen für 3- bis 6-Jährige. (Test review. BIKO screening for the development of basic skills for 3- to 6-year-olds.). Hogrefe Publishing Group, 2014. Epub ahead of print January 2014. https://doi.org/10.1026/0049-8637/a000122. Seeger D, Holodynski M, Souvignier E. Testbesprechung. BIKO-Screening zur Entwicklung von Basiskompetenzen für 3- bis 6-Jährige. (Test review. BIKO screening for the development of basic skills for 3- to 6-year-olds.). Hogrefe Publishing Group, 2014. Epub ahead of print January 2014. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1026/​0049-8637/​a000122.
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Tröster H, Flender J, Reineke D. Dortmunder Entwicklungsscreening für den Kindergarten (DESK 3–6). (Dortmund Development Screening for Kindergarten.). Kindheit und Entwicklung. 2005;14:140–9.CrossRef Tröster H, Flender J, Reineke D. Dortmunder Entwicklungsscreening für den Kindergarten (DESK 3–6). (Dortmund Development Screening for Kindergarten.). Kindheit und Entwicklung. 2005;14:140–9.CrossRef
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Bauer C, Krause M, Mayr T. Kompetenzen und Interessen von Kindern. Beobachtungs- und Einschätzboten für Kinder von 3,5 bis 6 Jahre. (Children’s competencies and interests. Observation and assessment tools for children from 3.5 to 6 years.). Gütersloh: Bertelsmann Stiftung; 2010. Bauer C, Krause M, Mayr T. Kompetenzen und Interessen von Kindern. Beobachtungs- und Einschätzboten für Kinder von 3,5 bis 6 Jahre. (Children’s competencies and interests. Observation and assessment tools for children from 3.5 to 6 years.). Gütersloh: Bertelsmann Stiftung; 2010.
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Janus M, Offord DR. Development and psychometric properties of the Early Development Instrument (EDI): A measure of children’s school readiness. Can J Behav Sci. 2007;39:1–22.CrossRef Janus M, Offord DR. Development and psychometric properties of the Early Development Instrument (EDI): A measure of children’s school readiness. Can J Behav Sci. 2007;39:1–22.CrossRef
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Georg S, Bosle C, Fischer JE, et al. Psychometric properties and contextual appropriateness of the German version of the Early Development Instrument. BMC Pediatr. 2020;20:339.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Georg S, Bosle C, Fischer JE, et al. Psychometric properties and contextual appropriateness of the German version of the Early Development Instrument. BMC Pediatr. 2020;20:339.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Brinkman SA, Gregory TA, Goldfeld S, et al. Data Resource Profile: The Australian Early Development Index (AEDI). Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43:1089–96.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Brinkman SA, Gregory TA, Goldfeld S, et al. Data Resource Profile: The Australian Early Development Index (AEDI). Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43:1089–96.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Hagquist C, Hellström L. The Psychometric Properties of the Early Development Instrument: A Rasch Analysis Based on Swedish Pilot Data. Soc Indic Res. 2013;117:301–17.CrossRef Hagquist C, Hellström L. The Psychometric Properties of the Early Development Instrument: A Rasch Analysis Based on Swedish Pilot Data. Soc Indic Res. 2013;117:301–17.CrossRef
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Woolfson LM, Geddes R, McNicol S, et al. A Cross-Sectional Pilot Study of the Scottish Early Development Instrument: A Tool for Addressing Inequality. BMC Public Health. 2013;13:1187.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Woolfson LM, Geddes R, McNicol S, et al. A Cross-Sectional Pilot Study of the Scottish Early Development Instrument: A Tool for Addressing Inequality. BMC Public Health. 2013;13:1187.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
52.
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Bailey R, Jones SM. An Integrated Model of Regulation for Applied Settings. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2019;22:2–23.PubMedCrossRef Bailey R, Jones SM. An Integrated Model of Regulation for Applied Settings. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2019;22:2–23.PubMedCrossRef
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Janus M. The Early Development Instrument: A Tool for Monitoring Children’s Development and Readiness for School. Early Child Development: From Measurement to Action A Priority for Growth and Equity. 2006. p. 141–55. Janus M. The Early Development Instrument: A Tool for Monitoring Children’s Development and Readiness for School. Early Child Development: From Measurement to Action A Priority for Growth and Equity. 2006. p. 141–55.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Hu L, Bentler PM, Hu L. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis : Conventional criteria versus new alternatives Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure Analysis : Conventional Criteria Versus New Alternatives. 5511. Epub ahead of print 2009. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705519909540118. Hu L, Bentler PM, Hu L. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis : Conventional criteria versus new alternatives Cutoff Criteria for Fit Indexes in Covariance Structure Analysis : Conventional Criteria Versus New Alternatives. 5511. Epub ahead of print 2009. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1080/​1070551990954011​8.
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Maccallum RC, Browne MW, Sugawara HM. Power Analysis and Determination of Sample Size for Covariance Structure Modeling of fit involving a particular measure of model. 1996;13:130–49. Maccallum RC, Browne MW, Sugawara HM. Power Analysis and Determination of Sample Size for Covariance Structure Modeling of fit involving a particular measure of model. 1996;13:130–49.
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Schumacker RE, Lomax RG. A Beginner’s Guide to Structural Equation Modeling. 4th ed. New York: Routledge; 2015.CrossRef Schumacker RE, Lomax RG. A Beginner’s Guide to Structural Equation Modeling. 4th ed. New York: Routledge; 2015.CrossRef
59.
Zurück zum Zitat Loehlin JC, Beaujean AA. Latent Variable Models: An Introduction to Factor, Path, and Structural Equation Analysis. New York: Taylor & Francis; 2017. Loehlin JC, Beaujean AA. Latent Variable Models: An Introduction to Factor, Path, and Structural Equation Analysis. New York: Taylor & Francis; 2017.
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Kyriazos TA. Applied Psychometrics: Sample Size and Sample Power Considerations in Factor Analysis (EFA, CFA) and SEM in General. Psychology. 2018;09:2207–30.CrossRef Kyriazos TA. Applied Psychometrics: Sample Size and Sample Power Considerations in Factor Analysis (EFA, CFA) and SEM in General. Psychology. 2018;09:2207–30.CrossRef
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Koo TK, Li MY. A Guideline of Selecting and Reporting Intraclass Correlation Coefficients for Reliability Research. J Chiropr Med. 2016;15:155–63.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Koo TK, Li MY. A Guideline of Selecting and Reporting Intraclass Correlation Coefficients for Reliability Research. J Chiropr Med. 2016;15:155–63.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Altman DG, Bland JM. Measurement in Medicine: The Analysis of Method Comparison Studies. The Statistician. 1983;32:307.CrossRef Altman DG, Bland JM. Measurement in Medicine: The Analysis of Method Comparison Studies. The Statistician. 1983;32:307.CrossRef
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Bland MJ, Altman DG. Applying the Right Statistics: Analyses of Measurement Studies. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2003;22:85–93.PubMedCrossRef Bland MJ, Altman DG. Applying the Right Statistics: Analyses of Measurement Studies. Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2003;22:85–93.PubMedCrossRef
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Savina E. Self-regulation in Preschool and Early Elementary Classrooms: Why It Is Important and How to Promote It. Early Childhood Educ J. 2021;49:493–501.CrossRef Savina E. Self-regulation in Preschool and Early Elementary Classrooms: Why It Is Important and How to Promote It. Early Childhood Educ J. 2021;49:493–501.CrossRef
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Kumah EA, Bettany-Saltikov J, van Schaik P, et al. Development and validation of a questionnaire to assess evidence-based practice and evidence-informed practice knowledge, attitudes, understanding and behavior. Teaching and Learning in Nursing. Epub ahead of print 2023. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.teln.2023.07.006. Kumah EA, Bettany-Saltikov J, van Schaik P, et al. Development and validation of a questionnaire to assess evidence-based practice and evidence-informed practice knowledge, attitudes, understanding and behavior. Teaching and Learning in Nursing. Epub ahead of print 2023. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1016/​j.​teln.​2023.​07.​006.
71.
Zurück zum Zitat Van Dijk M, Van Geert P. The nature and meaning of intraindividual variability in development in the early life span. In: Diehl M, Hooker K, Sliwinski MJ, editors. Handbook of intraindividual variablity across the life span. New York, East Sussex: Routledge; 2016. p. 37–58. Van Dijk M, Van Geert P. The nature and meaning of intraindividual variability in development in the early life span. In: Diehl M, Hooker K, Sliwinski MJ, editors. Handbook of intraindividual variablity across the life span. New York, East Sussex: Routledge; 2016. p. 37–58.
Metadaten
Titel
Development and validation of a self-regulation scale within the German version of the Early Development Instrument
verfasst von
Sabine Georg
Bernd Genser
Joachim Fischer
Steffi Sachse
Freia De Bock
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2023
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Pediatrics / Ausgabe 1/2023
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2431
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-023-04334-1

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2023

BMC Pediatrics 1/2023 Zur Ausgabe

Ähnliche Überlebensraten nach Reanimation während des Transports bzw. vor Ort

29.05.2024 Reanimation im Kindesalter Nachrichten

Laut einer Studie aus den USA und Kanada scheint es bei der Reanimation von Kindern außerhalb einer Klinik keinen Unterschied für das Überleben zu machen, ob die Wiederbelebungsmaßnahmen während des Transports in die Klinik stattfinden oder vor Ort ausgeführt werden. Jedoch gibt es dabei einige Einschränkungen und eine wichtige Ausnahme.

Alter der Mutter beeinflusst Risiko für kongenitale Anomalie

28.05.2024 Kinder- und Jugendgynäkologie Nachrichten

Welchen Einfluss das Alter ihrer Mutter auf das Risiko hat, dass Kinder mit nicht chromosomal bedingter Malformation zur Welt kommen, hat eine ungarische Studie untersucht. Sie zeigt: Nicht nur fortgeschrittenes Alter ist riskant.

Begünstigt Bettruhe der Mutter doch das fetale Wachstum?

Ob ungeborene Kinder, die kleiner als die meisten Gleichaltrigen sind, schneller wachsen, wenn die Mutter sich mehr ausruht, wird diskutiert. Die Ergebnisse einer US-Studie sprechen dafür.

Bei Amblyopie früher abkleben als bisher empfohlen?

22.05.2024 Fehlsichtigkeit Nachrichten

Bei Amblyopie ist das frühzeitige Abkleben des kontralateralen Auges in den meisten Fällen wohl effektiver als der Therapiestandard mit zunächst mehrmonatigem Brilletragen.

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.