Skip to main content
Erschienen in: European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 3/2023

Open Access 18.08.2022 | Short Communication

Discrepancy between high non-verbal intelligence and low accuracy at reading emotional expressions in the eyes reflects the magnitude of social–emotional difficulties in autism

verfasst von: Nouchine Hadjikhani, Martyna Galazka, Tal Kenet, Robert Joseph, Jakob Åsberg Johnels

Erschienen in: European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience | Ausgabe 3/2023

Abstract

Many so-called “high functioning” autistic individuals struggle with daily living skills, and have poorer than expected adult outcomes in employment, relationships, and quality of life. Significant discrepancies between non-verbal intelligence and emotional processing can be observed in autism, but the role of the magnitude of this gap in achieving potential psychosocial outcome is not known. Here, we show in a large group of participants (n = 107), that only among those with an autism diagnosis (n = 33), the gap between non-verbal intelligence (as measured by Raven’s matrices) and the ability to perform the Reading the Mind in the Eyes test significantly predicts self-perceived emotional/social difficulties as assessed by the Empathy Quotient. Our results suggest that it is specifically the magnitude of the gap between (high) levels of abstract reasoning skills and poor proficiency in reading emotions expressed by the eyes that predicts self-perceived difficulties in emotional and social interactions among adults with autism. A better understanding of the underlying causes of the discrepancy between potential and actual psychosocial outcomes is the first step toward developing the most appropriate support for this vulnerable population, and our study offers some potentially important insights in this regard.
Hinweise

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s00406-022-01471-z.

Introduction

Autism is a lifelong condition, and children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) grow into adults with ASD. While many individuals with ASD require lifelong support, those with average and above-average general intelligence (IQ), i.e., without intellectual disability (referred to from now on as ASDnoID), are generally considered to be “high functioning”. However, despite terminology, many ASDnoID individuals do not have adult outcomes commensurate with their IQ. Several studies report that many so-called “high functioning” ASD individuals show a gap between cognitive skills and adaptive behavior, and struggle with everyday functions (e.g., [1, 2, 8, 11, 1317, 20, 26, 31]). For example, a large number of studies on occupational activities in adults with ASD (reviewed in [16]) show that a considerable proportion of adults with above-average IQ are unemployed or underemployed, that salary of employed adults with ASD is significantly lower than for adults with other disabilities, and that despite average levels of education, ASDnoID adults have high rates of financial dependency [28], and poorer quality of life (QoL) [22, 25].
Causes of the poorer than expected outcomes in a proportion of autistic individuals who do not have any intellectual disability are still not well understood. One of the underlying mechanisms could have its origins in the greater difficulty experienced when deciphering emotions expressed by others—an ability that largely relies on extracting information from the eye region. Yet, it is well known that many autistic individuals tend to avoid looking at eyes [29, 32].
The Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET) [4] measures the ability to understand others’ emotional state based on the picture of the eyes of an actor. It has been shown that autistic individuals have worse performance than typical controls on the RMET ([3, 4]). It has also been shown that RMET scores are positively related to emotional empathy [6]. The Empathy Quotient (EQ) is a self-measure of cognitive empathy, emotional reactivity, and social skills [18], and low RMET accuracy has been associated with low EQ scores [6, 9].
What has not been examined is the experience of falling relatively much below on RMET relative to one’s own general cognitive capacity. Interestingly, research on another neurodevelopmental difficulty (dyslexia) has shown that the dyslexic impairment is seen as particularly disabling for those individuals with high general cognitive skills, potentially due to the discrepancy between reading attainment and the higher personal and external expectations for this population [10]. In autism, it is possible that a similar discrepancy between high cognitive skills and low social/emotional skills results in higher self-perceived disability, and greater challenges in daily living.
Here, we examined performance on the RMET together with performance on Raven’s Progressive Matrices (RPM) [27], a non-verbal test of abstract reasoning in a group of autistic individuals (n = 33) and typical controls (n = 74). We used the Empathy Quotient (EQ) test to assess self-perceived everyday social–emotional difficulties [18]. Our goal was to investigate whether the discrepancy between non-verbal intelligence (RPM) and accuracy at reading emotional expressions in the eyes (RMET) would predict the magnitude of self-perceived social–emotional difficulties measured with the EQ in ASD and in controls.

Participants and methods

Sample description

All procedures were in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the University Hospital ethics committee. Written informed consent was obtained from all adult participants and from all parents of participating adolescents. All adolescents also gave their oral assent. The total sample size was 107. Among those, 33 (3 females) were clinically diagnosed with ASD using DSM IV-TR criteria in combination with either the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS [21]) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R [23] 1994), or the Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorder (DISCO [19]). None of the 74 controls (CON, 13 females) had a history of psychiatric/neurological disorders. See Table 1.
Table 1
Descriptive statistics of all the variables measured
Variable
ASD (n = 33,3F)
Controls (n = 74,13F)
Group difference
Age ± SD (range)
22.3 ± 7.91 (14.1–41.7)
22.9 ± 6.77 (14.1–41.9)
p = 0.703, d = − 0.083
RPM score ± SD (range)
10.2 ± 1.81 (5–12)
10.7 ± 1.30 (7–12)
p = 0.123, d = − 0.348
RMET score ± SD (range)
21.9 ± 4.87 (11–31)
24.9 ± 2.93 (18–31)
p = 0.002, d = − 0.753
EQ ± SD (range)
24.5 ± 8.35 (9–41)
38.9 ± 9.60 (23–67)
p < 0.001, d = − 1.596
Discrepancy score ± SD (range)
0.285 ± 1.60 (− 3.48 to 2.87)
− 0.127 ± 1.05 (− 3.02 to 2.25)
p = 0.183, d = 0.304
Significant differences are in bold
RPM Raven’s progressive matrices, RMET reading the mind in the eyes test, EQ empathy quotient, d Cohen’s effect size

Statistical analyses

Differences between groups were performed using Welch’s t tests to account for the difference in sample size and in differential variance between the two groups. To compute the discrepancy between RPM and RMET scores, data were first z-transformed, and for each participant we computed the value of the discrepancy score [(z-RPM)-(z-RMET)]. A bivariate linear regression was conducted to examine how well the discrepancy could predict EQ for each group, separately.

Results

Group differences are presented in Table 1. ASD and control participants did not differ in age or in IQ as assessed by the RPM. However, as expected, ASD had significantly lower scores on the RMET (p = 0.002, d = − 0.833) and EQ (p < 0.001, d = − 1.555). Groups did not differ in the discrepancy score computed as explained above (p = 0.183, d = 0.304).

Linear regressions

Main analysis: discrepancy score and EQ (fig. 1)

For the ASD group, the correlation between discrepancy score and EQ was statistically significant, r(31) = 0.664, p < 0.001, with a large effect size [5]. The regression equation for predicting EQ was Y = 25.5–3.46*X. The r2 for this equation was 0.441; that is, 44.1% of the variance in EQ was predictable from the discrepancy score.
For the control group, the correlation between discrepancy score and EQ was not statistically significant, r(72) = 0.076, p = 0.522. The regression equation for predicting EQ was Y = 38.97 + 0.69*X. The r2 for this equation was 0.006; that is, only 0.6% of the variance in EQ was predictable from the discrepancy score.
Additional correlations were also examined.
RPM and RMET: The correlation between RPM and RMET score was not statistically significant for either group (ASD: r(31) = 0.159, p = 0.378; CON: r(72) = 0.170, p = 0.148).
RMET and EQ: The correlation between the RMET score and EQ was significant for the ASD group: r(31) = 0.379, p = 0.029. It was not significant for the control group: r(72) = 0.074, p = 0.528. The r2 for the regression equation was 0.144, meaning that 14.4% of the variance in EQ was explained by the RMET score in the ASD group, which is less than the 44.1% of the variance in EQ explained by the gap between the RPM and the RMET scores.
RPM and EQ: The correlation between RPM and EQ was also significant for the ASD group: r(31) = 0.484, p = 0.004, and it was not significant for the control group: r(72) = 0.156, p = 0.185. The r2 for the regression equation was 0.234, meaning that 23.4% of the variance in EQ was explained by RPM in the ASD group, which is less than the 44.1% of the variance in EQ explained by the gap between the RPM and the RMET scores.
The gap score was, therefore, the best predictor of the variance in EQ (Fig. 1).

Discussion

Causes of the poorer than expected outcome in a proportion of autistic individuals who do not have intellectual disability are still not well understood. Here, we show that contrary to what is observed in typical individuals, where the variability in socio-emotional processing cannot be explained by the difference between abstract reasoning and emotion understanding, autistic individuals report significantly more difficulties in cognitive empathy, emotional reactivity and social skills as the gap between these two constructs deepens. Our results suggests that it is specifically the magnitude of the gap between (high) levels of abstract reasoning skills and poor proficiency in reading emotions expressed by the eyes that predicts such self-perceived difficulties in emotional and social interactions among adults with autism. In terms of possible explanations, at an individual level, some autistic individuals find looking at the eyes of others aversive, as indicated by heightened amygdala activation during direct eye contact [12, 24, 30, 32], for review, see [29]. Because the eyes contain a lot of information regarding emotions and intentions, eye contact avoiding may result in reduced expertise, and in increased difficulties in social processing and emotional understanding.

Limitations

We did not collect broader measures of adaptive behaviors such as the Vineland, QoL, or executive functions in this group of participants, and relied on self-reported questionnaire (EQ).

Conclusion

The WHO defines QoL as “an individual’s perception of their position in life in the context of the culture and value systems in which they live and in relation to their goals, expectations, standards and concerns” [2]. A number of studies consistently report worse QoL in ASDnoID individuals (e.g., [7, 25]), but the underlying causes are not completely understood. We propose that one of the predictors of poorer than expected outcome in this population could be explained by the discrepancy between intact non-verbal intelligence and difficulties in socio-cognitive skills, that may be the result of eye avoidance used to self-regulate hyperarousal. There is a need for a more precise understanding of the relationship between issues related to social processing and/or to sensory processing and reduced QoL among autistic individuals, and how these may be experienced differently depending on cognitive capacities.

Acknowledgements

The authors want to acknowledge Nicole Zürcher Wimmer and  Loyse Hippolyte for their help in data collection. This work is supported by Swiss National Foundation grant # PPP00P3-130191 to NH, and by Swedish Vetenskapsrådet grant 2018-02397 to NH.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Open AccessThis article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Unsere Produktempfehlungen

e.Med Interdisziplinär

Kombi-Abonnement

Jetzt e.Med zum Sonderpreis bestellen!

Für Ihren Erfolg in Klinik und Praxis - Die beste Hilfe in Ihrem Arbeitsalltag

Mit e.Med Interdisziplinär erhalten Sie Zugang zu allen CME-Fortbildungen und Fachzeitschriften auf SpringerMedizin.de.

Jetzt bestellen und 100 € sparen!

Neuer Inhalt

e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie

Kombi-Abonnement

Mit e.Med Neurologie & Psychiatrie erhalten Sie Zugang zu CME-Fortbildungen der Fachgebiete, den Premium-Inhalten der dazugehörigen Fachzeitschriften, inklusive einer gedruckten Zeitschrift Ihrer Wahl.

Weitere Produktempfehlungen anzeigen
Anhänge

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Literatur
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson DK, Maye MP, Lord C (2011) Changes in maladaptive behaviors from midchildhood to young adulthood in autism spectrum disorder. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil 116:381–397CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Anderson DK, Maye MP, Lord C (2011) Changes in maladaptive behaviors from midchildhood to young adulthood in autism spectrum disorder. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil 116:381–397CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Baron-Cohen S, Bowen DC, Holt RJ, Allison C, Auyeung B, Lombardo MV, Smith P, Lai MC (2015) The “reading the mind in the eyes” test: complete absence of typical sex difference in ~400 men and women with autism. PLoS One 10:e0136521CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Baron-Cohen S, Bowen DC, Holt RJ, Allison C, Auyeung B, Lombardo MV, Smith P, Lai MC (2015) The “reading the mind in the eyes” test: complete absence of typical sex difference in ~400 men and women with autism. PLoS One 10:e0136521CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I (2001) The “reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 42:241–251CrossRefPubMed Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I (2001) The “reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 42:241–251CrossRefPubMed
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Demers LA, Koven NS (2015) The relation of alexithymic traits to affective theory of mind. Am J Psychol 128:31–42CrossRefPubMed Demers LA, Koven NS (2015) The relation of alexithymic traits to affective theory of mind. Am J Psychol 128:31–42CrossRefPubMed
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Dijkhuis RR, Ziermans TB, Van Rijn S, Staal WG, Swaab H (2017) Self-regulation and quality of life in high-functioning young adults with autism. Autism 21:896–906CrossRefPubMed Dijkhuis RR, Ziermans TB, Van Rijn S, Staal WG, Swaab H (2017) Self-regulation and quality of life in high-functioning young adults with autism. Autism 21:896–906CrossRefPubMed
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Duncan AW, Bishop SL (2015) Understanding the gap between cognitive abilities and daily living skills in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders with average intelligence. Autism 19:64–72CrossRefPubMed Duncan AW, Bishop SL (2015) Understanding the gap between cognitive abilities and daily living skills in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders with average intelligence. Autism 19:64–72CrossRefPubMed
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Elbro C (2010) Dyslexia as disability or handicap: When does vocabulary matter? J Learn Disabil 43:469–478CrossRefPubMed Elbro C (2010) Dyslexia as disability or handicap: When does vocabulary matter? J Learn Disabil 43:469–478CrossRefPubMed
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Gilotty L, Kenworthy L, Sirian L, Black DO, Wagner AE (2002) Adaptive skills and executive function in autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychol 8:241–248CrossRefPubMed Gilotty L, Kenworthy L, Sirian L, Black DO, Wagner AE (2002) Adaptive skills and executive function in autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychol 8:241–248CrossRefPubMed
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Hadjikhani N, ÅsbergJohnels J, Lassalle A, Zürcher NR, Hippolyte L, Gillberg C, Lemonnier E, Ben-Ari Y (2018) Bumetanide for autism: more eye contact, less amygdala activation. Sci Rep 8:3602CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Hadjikhani N, ÅsbergJohnels J, Lassalle A, Zürcher NR, Hippolyte L, Gillberg C, Lemonnier E, Ben-Ari Y (2018) Bumetanide for autism: more eye contact, less amygdala activation. Sci Rep 8:3602CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Howlin P, Mawhood L, Rutter M (2000) Autism and developmental receptive language disorder–a follow-up comparison in early adult life. Ii: social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 41:561–578CrossRefPubMed Howlin P, Mawhood L, Rutter M (2000) Autism and developmental receptive language disorder–a follow-up comparison in early adult life. Ii: social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 41:561–578CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Kanne SM, Gerber AJ, Quirmbach LM, Sparrow SS, Cicchetti DV, Saulnier CA (2011) The role of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorders: Implications for functional outcome. J Autism Dev Disord 41:1007–1018CrossRefPubMed Kanne SM, Gerber AJ, Quirmbach LM, Sparrow SS, Cicchetti DV, Saulnier CA (2011) The role of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorders: Implications for functional outcome. J Autism Dev Disord 41:1007–1018CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Kenworthy L, Case L, Harms MB, Martin A, Wallace GL (2010) Adaptive behavior ratings correlate with symptomatology and iq among individuals with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 40:416–423CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Kenworthy L, Case L, Harms MB, Martin A, Wallace GL (2010) Adaptive behavior ratings correlate with symptomatology and iq among individuals with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 40:416–423CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Klin A, Saulnier CA, Sparrow SS, Cicchetti DV, Volkmar FR, Lord C (2007) Social and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: the vineland and the ados. J Autism Dev Disord 37:748–759CrossRefPubMed Klin A, Saulnier CA, Sparrow SS, Cicchetti DV, Volkmar FR, Lord C (2007) Social and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: the vineland and the ados. J Autism Dev Disord 37:748–759CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Kraper CK, Kenworthy L, Popal H, Martin A, Wallace GL (2017) The gap between adaptive behavior and intelligence in autism persists into young adulthood and is linked to psychiatric co-morbidities. J Autism Dev Disord 47:3007–3017CrossRefPubMed Kraper CK, Kenworthy L, Popal H, Martin A, Wallace GL (2017) The gap between adaptive behavior and intelligence in autism persists into young adulthood and is linked to psychiatric co-morbidities. J Autism Dev Disord 47:3007–3017CrossRefPubMed
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Lawrence EJ, Shaw P, Baker D, Baron-Cohen S, David AS (2004) Measuring empathy: reliability and validity of the empathy quotient. Psychol Med 34:911–919CrossRefPubMed Lawrence EJ, Shaw P, Baker D, Baron-Cohen S, David AS (2004) Measuring empathy: reliability and validity of the empathy quotient. Psychol Med 34:911–919CrossRefPubMed
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Leekam SR, Libby SJ, Wing L, Gould J, Taylor C (2002) The diagnostic interview for social and communication disorders: algorithms for icd-10 childhood autism and wing and gould autistic spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 43:327–342CrossRefPubMed Leekam SR, Libby SJ, Wing L, Gould J, Taylor C (2002) The diagnostic interview for social and communication disorders: algorithms for icd-10 childhood autism and wing and gould autistic spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 43:327–342CrossRefPubMed
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Liss M, Harel B, Fein D, Allen D, Dunn M, Feinstein C, Morris R, Waterhouse L, Rapin I (2001) Predictors and correlates of adaptive functioning in children with developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 31:219–230CrossRefPubMed Liss M, Harel B, Fein D, Allen D, Dunn M, Feinstein C, Morris R, Waterhouse L, Rapin I (2001) Predictors and correlates of adaptive functioning in children with developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 31:219–230CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH Jr, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, Pickles A, Rutter M (2000) The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord 30:205–223CrossRefPubMed Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH Jr, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, Pickles A, Rutter M (2000) The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord 30:205–223CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop SL (2012) Autism diagnostic observation schedule (ados-2), 2nd edn. Western Psychological Services, Torrence, CA Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop SL (2012) Autism diagnostic observation schedule (ados-2), 2nd edn. Western Psychological Services, Torrence, CA
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A (1994) Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 24:659–685CrossRefPubMed Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A (1994) Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 24:659–685CrossRefPubMed
24.
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Otsuka S, Uono S, Yoshimura S, Zhao S, Toichi M (2017) Emotion perception mediates the predictive relationship between verbal ability and functional outcome in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 47:1166–1182CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Otsuka S, Uono S, Yoshimura S, Zhao S, Toichi M (2017) Emotion perception mediates the predictive relationship between verbal ability and functional outcome in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord 47:1166–1182CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Pugliese CE, Anthony LG, Strang JF, Dudley K, Wallace GL, Naiman DQ, Kenworthy L (2016) Longitudinal examination of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorders: Influence of executive function. J Autism Dev Disord 46:467–477CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Pugliese CE, Anthony LG, Strang JF, Dudley K, Wallace GL, Naiman DQ, Kenworthy L (2016) Longitudinal examination of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorders: Influence of executive function. J Autism Dev Disord 46:467–477CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH (2003) Manual for raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Section 4: the advanced progressive matrices. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment Raven J, Raven JC, Court JH (2003) Manual for raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Section 4: the advanced progressive matrices. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Saulnier CA, Klin A (2007) Brief report: social and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals with autism and asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 37:788–793CrossRefPubMed Saulnier CA, Klin A (2007) Brief report: social and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals with autism and asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 37:788–793CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Tillmann J, San Jose Caceres A, Chatham CH, Crawley D, Holt R, Oakley B, Banaschewski T, Baron-Cohen S, Bolte S, Buitelaar JK, Durston S, Ham L, Loth E, Simonoff E, Spooren W, Murphy DG, Charman T, group E-AL (2019) Investigating the factors underlying adaptive functioning in autism in the eu-aims longitudinal european autism project. Autism Res 12:645–657CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Tillmann J, San Jose Caceres A, Chatham CH, Crawley D, Holt R, Oakley B, Banaschewski T, Baron-Cohen S, Bolte S, Buitelaar JK, Durston S, Ham L, Loth E, Simonoff E, Spooren W, Murphy DG, Charman T, group E-AL (2019) Investigating the factors underlying adaptive functioning in autism in the eu-aims longitudinal european autism project. Autism Res 12:645–657CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Trevisan DA, Roberts N, Lin C, Birmingham E (2017) How do adults and teens with self-declared autism spectrum disorder experience eye contact? A qualitative analysis of first-hand accounts. PLoS One 12:e0188446CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Trevisan DA, Roberts N, Lin C, Birmingham E (2017) How do adults and teens with self-declared autism spectrum disorder experience eye contact? A qualitative analysis of first-hand accounts. PLoS One 12:e0188446CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
Metadaten
Titel
Discrepancy between high non-verbal intelligence and low accuracy at reading emotional expressions in the eyes reflects the magnitude of social–emotional difficulties in autism
verfasst von
Nouchine Hadjikhani
Martyna Galazka
Tal Kenet
Robert Joseph
Jakob Åsberg Johnels
Publikationsdatum
18.08.2022
Verlag
Springer Berlin Heidelberg
Erschienen in
European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience / Ausgabe 3/2023
Print ISSN: 0940-1334
Elektronische ISSN: 1433-8491
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-022-01471-z

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 3/2023

European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 3/2023 Zur Ausgabe

Hörschwäche erhöht Demenzrisiko unabhängig von Beta-Amyloid

29.05.2024 Hörstörungen Nachrichten

Hört jemand im Alter schlecht, nimmt das Hirn- und Hippocampusvolumen besonders schnell ab, was auch mit einem beschleunigten kognitiven Abbau einhergeht. Und diese Prozesse scheinen sich unabhängig von der Amyloidablagerung zu ereignen.

So wirken verschiedene Alkoholika auf den Blutdruck

23.05.2024 Störungen durch Alkohol Nachrichten

Je mehr Alkohol Menschen pro Woche trinken, desto mehr steigt ihr Blutdruck, legen Daten aus Dänemark nahe. Ob es dabei auch auf die Art des Alkohols ankommt, wurde ebenfalls untersucht.

„Übersichtlicher Wegweiser“: Lauterbachs umstrittener Klinik-Atlas ist online

17.05.2024 Klinik aktuell Nachrichten

Sie sei „ethisch geboten“, meint Gesundheitsminister Karl Lauterbach: mehr Transparenz über die Qualität von Klinikbehandlungen. Um sie abzubilden, lässt er gegen den Widerstand vieler Länder einen virtuellen Klinik-Atlas freischalten.

ADHS-Medikation erhöht das kardiovaskuläre Risiko

16.05.2024 Herzinsuffizienz Nachrichten

Erwachsene, die Medikamente gegen das Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-Hyperaktivitätssyndrom einnehmen, laufen offenbar erhöhte Gefahr, an Herzschwäche zu erkranken oder einen Schlaganfall zu erleiden. Es scheint eine Dosis-Wirkungs-Beziehung zu bestehen.

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.