Skip to main content
Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics 1/2023

Open Access 01.12.2023 | Study protocol

Effects of a developmental dyslexia remediation protocol based on the training of audio-phonological cognitive processes in dyslexic children with high intellectual potential: study protocol for a multiple-baseline single-case experimental design

verfasst von: Gaëlle Darrot, Auriane Gros, Valeria Manera, Bruno De Cara, Sylvane Faure, Xavier Corveleyn, Karine Harrar-Eskinazi

Erschienen in: BMC Pediatrics | Ausgabe 1/2023

Abstract

Background

The significant prevalence of children with high intellectual potential (HIP) in the school-age population and the high rate of comorbidity with learning disabilities such as dyslexia has increased the demand for speech and language therapy and made it more complex. However, the management of dyslexic patients with high intellectual potential (HIP-DD) is poorly referenced in the literature. A large majority of studies on HIP-DD children focus on the screening and diagnosis of developmental dyslexia, but only a few address remediation. Developmental dyslexia is a severe and persistent disorder that affects the acquisition of reading and implies the impairment of several underlying cognitive processes. These include deficits in Categorical Perception, Rapid Automatized Naming, and phonological awareness, particularly phonemic awareness. Some authors claim that HIP-DD children's underlying deficits mainly concern rapid automatized naming and phonological awareness. Thus, the purpose of this study is to present a remediation protocol for developmental dyslexia in HIP-DD children. This protocol proposes to compare the effects on reading skills of an intensive intervention targeting categorical perception, rapid automatized naming, and phonemic analysis versus standard speech therapy remediation in HIP-DD children.

Methods

A multiple-baseline single-case experimental design (A1BCA2) will be proposed to 4 French HIP-DD patients for a period of 30 weeks. Intervention phases B and C correspond to categorical perception training and rapid automatized naming training. During phases B and C, each training session will be associated with phonemic analysis training and a reading and writing task. At inclusion, a speech and language, psychological, and neuropsychological assessment will be performed to define the four patients' profiles. Patients will be assigned to the different baseline lengths using a simple computerized randomization procedure. The duration of the phases will be counterbalanced. The study will be double blinded. A weekly measurement of phonological and reading skills will be performed for the full duration of the study.

Discussion

The purpose of this protocol is to observe the evolution of reading skills with each type of intervention. From this observation, hypotheses concerning the remediation of developmental dyslexia in HIP-DD children can be tested. The strengths and limitations of the study are discussed.

Trial registration

ClinicalTrials.gov, NCT04028310. Registered on July 18, 2019. Version identifier is no. ID RCB 2019-A01453-54, 19-HPNCL-02, 07/18/2019.
Hinweise

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Abkürzungen
HIP
High intellectual potential
HIP-DD
Dyslexic with high intellectual potential
IQ
Intellectual Quotient
DD
Developmental Dyslexia
UCD
Underlying Cognitive Deficit(s)
RAN
Rapid Automatized Naming
CP
Categorical Perception
PA
Phonemic Analysis
CV
Consonant/Vowel
CCV
Consonant/Consonant/Vowel
VOT
Voice Onset Time
SCED
Single-Case Experimental Design
EBP
Evidence Based Practice

Background

Studies on learning disabilities in children with high intellectual potential (HIP) are underrepresented in the scientific literature, although their number has been increasing gradually over the years. However, the high prevalence of HIP children in the school-age population, approximately 2.3% of French school children [1], and the high rate of comorbidity with learning disabilities has increased the demand for speech and language therapy care. Some studies claim that the prevalence of HIP among dyslexic readers is higher than the prevalence of HIP among normal readers. For instance, Toffalini [2] and al. reported a proportion of 5.06% HIP children among dyslexic readers compared to 1.82% HIP children among normal readers. Pradeille [3] indicates a proportion of 10% HIP children within a sample of 209 dyslexic readers. According to Winner [4], dyslexia is the most frequent developmental disorder associated with HIP. However, the diagnosis and rehabilitation of children with a high intellectual potential and developmental dyslexia (HIP-DD) remains difficult due to the co-existence of both cognitive features.
Individuals with high intellectual potential represent a small percentage of the population whose intelligence quotient (IQ) is higher than 130 on the Weschler intelligence scale, Wisc IV [5]. However, some authors question the total IQ measure as the only diagnostic tool for these individuals [510]. Indeed, the diagnosis of high intellectual potential cannot be limited to the measurement of their IQ, but must also consider the specific characteristics of their brain’s operation. Besides the high IQ score, an HIP child has specific intellectual characteristics, as well as a different cognitive organization [11, 12]. Some authors discuss the presence of specific psycho-affective characteristics in HIP individuals, but there is no consensus on this topic in the literature. Despite this, there are objectively detectable differences on a purely cerebral level. The cerebral particularities of HIP children have been identified in many neuroimaging studies. An increased transmission of information between the left temporal and left central regions, between the left temporal and left parietal regions, and between the left central and left parietal regions has been demonstrated in individuals with HIP compared to neurotypical individuals during a scientific hypothesis generation task [13]. Thus, there appears to be a different distribution of brain activity in HIP individuals, who seem to more efficiently distribute the cognitive resources needed for hypothesis generation [13]. The brains of HIP individuals also differ from neurotypical ones in their strength; there is greater activation of certain brain areas in HIP children during certain cognitive tasks, especially in the prefrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate cortex, and the posterior parietal cortex [14]. In 2021, Christoph Fraenz demonstrated that the strength of neuronal connections at rest between frontal and parietal regions correlates with performance in reasoning tests such as those used in IQ tests [15]. Thus, a higher IQ may mean better quality connections between frontal and parietal regions. Differences in connection between the two hemispheres of the brain were also found in the 2007 study by Luders et al. who found a link between corpus callosum thickness and intelligence scores in adulthood [16]. In addition, numerous studies demonstrate a brain volume proportional to IQ, with a consistent increase in volume as IQ increases [17]. Regarding possible emotional and psycho-affective particularities, there is no consensus in the scientific literature.
Concerning developmental dyslexia (DD), the DSM-V defines it as a severe and persistent disorder that affects reading acquisition, despite a normal intelligence and the absence of neurological or psychiatric pathology, visual and auditory sensory deficits, or socio-educational deficiencies [18]. Developmental dyslexia is a neurodevelopmental disorder that disturbs the automatization of associations between written linguistic units (graphemes) and oral linguistic units (phonemes) [19]. This alphabetic decoding disorder leads to an identification disorder of written words, which can have severe consequences on school learning, on daily and professional life, and on the self-esteem of the patients. The phonological causal hypothesis is currently the most widespread [20]. However, over the last few years, the single cause theory has been criticized in favor of a multi-factorial causal theory [2123] involving multiple underlying cognitive deficits (UCD) [24, 25]. The two types of UCD most described in the literature are audio-phonological deficits and visuo-attentional deficits. In 2019, Ziegler et al. [26] acknowledged the multifactorial nature of dyslexia but concluded that phonological deficits tended to prevail over the other types of UCD. According to Saksida's study [20], in the non-HIP dyslexic population, the most frequent UCD concerns phonological awareness (PA) and rapid automatized naming (RAN). PA is a cognitive process that allows individuals to manipulate language syllables, rhymes, or phonemes [27]. In the dyslexic population, it is mainly the manipulation of phonemes that is affected [28] and is thus referred to as phonemic awareness. RAN refers to the rapid retrieval of phonological information from a word presented as a picture, followed by its immediate oral production. RAN and phonological awareness are two distinct phonological processes directly involved in dyslexia [29]. In a more recent study [30], phonological awareness was identified as a predictor of word decoding skills and RAN as a predictor of word recognition and reading comprehension. Other researchers agree that the phonological processing deficit in dyslexia is the result of a more fundamental deficit in the perceptual processing of auditory information. Indeed, some dyslexic readers have an allophonic perception of speech sounds which leads to a poor discrimination of acoustic differences, a perceptual deficit in phoneme categorization and consequently a phonological processing deficit. Increased sensitivity to phonemic contrasts between variants of the same phoneme seems to lead to disorders in phoneme categorical perception (CP) [31]. Thus, instead of perceiving sounds in phonemic units, dyslexic readers could perceive them in allophonic units [32].
Concerning HIP-DD diagnosis, the DSM-V reports that compensatory strategies implemented by HIP-DD children may delay the onset of reading disorders and therefore prevent early diagnosis. Indeed, the cognitive profile of HIP-DD children seems to differ from that of HIP-normal readers, but also from that of dyslexic children without HIP [33, 34]. Their double cognitive particularity, HIP and dyslexia, often complicates the interpretation of scores obtained on IQ subtests, but also the interpretation of scores obtained on diagnostic tasks for DD. HIP-DD children, because of their reading difficulties, rarely reach the cut-off score of 130 IQ points determined as an indicator of high intellectual potential. For some authors, it is even meaningless to calculate an IQ when the patient has dyslexia [33]. Similarly, because of their HIP, dyslexic readers sometimes obtain non-pathological scores on the reading and writing tasks used to diagnose dyslexia [35, 36]. The diagnosis of HIP DD children is made difficult by reading and spelling performances that fall between the performances of non-HIP normo-readers and the performances of non-dyslexic HIP [35]. The diagnosis of dyslexia in an HIP child is therefore delayed because of performances that are considered normal by some tests that were not standardized on an HIP population. With their protective factors, HIP children should score above average on some subtests. Thus, when their scores are only average, it often means that their results are actually below what should be expected of them. Several authors describe a phenomenon of "discrepancy" [37, 38], in which dyslexia overshadows HIP and HIP masks dyslexia [33]. So, the cognitive profile of HIP DD is characterized both by phonological deficits related to dyslexia and by often higher verbal and visual-spatial working memory skills, as well as richer vocabulary and grammar than non-HIP readers [34]. Moreover, the study conducted by Van Viersen [35] asserts that the HIP-DD children's UCD primarily concern RAN and phonological awareness. Targeted interventions on RAN or phonological awareness significantly improve reading skills in dyslexic populations [39]. We also know that phonological awareness training combined with print knowledge is more effective than phonological awareness training alone [40, 41]. The effects of RAN training on reading performances have also been proven in a study published in 2019 [42]. Also, an intervention on categorical perception of French phonemes has shown effects on phonemic awareness and reading skills in dyslexic readers [43]. However, remediation methods focused on PA, RAN and CP have not been tested on the specific population of HIP-DD children. Given the large number of differences in brain function between HIP and non-HIP, it would therefore seem appropriate to conduct more studies to improve the therapy of these "twice-exceptional" children. This term refers to individuals with a double particularity; on the one hand, a high intellectual potential, and on the other hand, a comorbid learning disability [44]. Thus, the purpose of this study is to present a single-case experimental protocol proposing an intervention on phonological processes in dyslexic children with high intellectual potential. The detailed methods of this intervention are derived from an ongoing group study evaluating the effectiveness of a UCD intervention on dyslexic children aged 8 to 13 years [45]. The training programs described in this protocol are tailored to the cognitive profile of each participant and can therefore be applied to HIP-DD children.

Methods and design

Study design choice

To meet our objectives, we propose an experimental protocol in the form of a single-case multiple-baseline design (A1-B-C-A2) across 4 patients. Phase A1 represents the baseline, which constitutes the patient's initial state, and during which the participant attends standard speech therapy sessions weekly, without intensive training. Phases B and C represent the intervention phases in which remediation is performed [46]: categorical perception (CP) and rapid automatized naming (RAN) are each associated with a phonemic analysis (PA) task. Finally, as it is impossible to return to the A1 baseline because of the expected ongoing benefits of the intervention, the A2 phase represents a post-intervention phase. During the A2 phase, the patient no longer undergoes intensive training but continues to attend standard speech therapy sessions, and the judgment criteria continue to be measured [47]. In cognitive rehabilitation studies, the multiple-baseline single-case experimental design is considered particularly suitable [48]. Several patients with similar cognitive profiles are assigned the same protocol, but the baseline duration differs between them. This additional control ensures that the effect obtained in the B or C phase is attributable to the intervention and that the baseline trend would remain stable in the absence of intervention, which excludes a possible temporal and/or session number bias [49]. The interventions are combined in the form A-B-C without any intermediate baseline return in order to compare the separate effects of each intervention on the studied variable [47]. To control the independent effectiveness of both the B and C interventions [49], a crossover between patients will be achieved by changing the order of interventions. Thus, with four patients, the training order will be counterbalanced so that two of the patients start with PC training, and the two others start with RAN training.

Recruitment and study population

Population

Four French children with a diagnosis of developmental dyslexia and HIP will be recruited for this study. Participants will be enrolled by the principal investigator in the context of her clinical practice. Patients will therefore be informed of the possibility of participating in the study during their regular speech and language therapy appointments. After clarifying the protocol, the speech and language therapist will provide information sheets for the parents and children. A slide presentation will be shown to explain in a simple and entertaining way how the intervention can modify the cognitive processes involved in HIP-DD. Participants and their legal guardians will sign an information sheet and complete an informed consent form before entering the protocol. The inclusion and exclusion criteria are presented in Table 1.
Table 1
Inclusion, non-inclusion, and exclusion criteria
Inclusion criteria
Non-inclusion criteria
Exclusion criteria
- Age ≥ 8 years and ≤ 13 years
- Diagnosis of HIP confirmed by standardized tests and neuropsychological expertise
- Diagnosis of dyslexia confirmed by performance ≤—1.5 standard deviations on leximetry tests compared to the child's developmental age
- Performance ≤ -1.5 standard deviations from the norm on phonological tasks in phonological tests
- Ownership of a personal computer and an internet connection
- Signature of an informed consent form by the parents or legal guardian
- Social insurance coverage
- Intellectual deficiency
- Neurological Disorders
- Pervasive Developmental Disorder
- Primary Sensory Impairment
- Educational deficits
- ADHD
- Specific oral language disorders
- Previous exposure to intensive daily phonological training
- Bilingualism
- Non-compliance with the protocol
- Withdrawal from the experiment upon request of the subject or his/her legal guardian

Pre-test and recruitment

The four participants will be matched in age and will have a similar cognitive profile. This will be achieved by conducting an inclusion assessment consisting of a complete set of tests assessing written language and underlying cognitive deficits of reading (see Table 2). This assessment will take place in three one-hour sessions. The inclusion assessment will determine the participants' selection before the A1 phase. The assessment procedure is the same as for the group study described in the Harrar-Eskinazi et al. study [45].
Table 2
An overview of the assessment batteries [45]
Cognitive Processes
Measures
Software
Reading and spelling assessments
Reading aloud
Meaningless text reading
Alouette©
Meaningful text reading
Evaléo©
2-min word reading
Evaléo©: Eval2M
Regular, irregular, pseudo-word reading
Evalec©
Reading comprehension
Multiple choice statements
Orlec 3©
Spelling
Phonetic, lexical and grammatical spelling
Chronosdictées©
Underlying cognitive process assessments
Phonological process
Phonological analysis
Evalec©
Phonological short-term memory
Evalec©
Rapid automatized naming
Evalec©
Visual-attentional process
Visual-attentional span
Evadys©
Global/local analysis
Sigl©
Complementary assessments
Span memory
Digit span
Evaléo©
Visual-spatial span
Corsi©
Oral language
Vocabulary and syntactic comprehension
Evaléo©
E.co.s.se©

Reading and spelling assessment

Reading aloud

Non-significant text
Alouette© [50, 51] is a test (265 words) considered as the gold standard of leximetry tests. It evaluates speed and accuracy when reading a meaningless text. This test provides a reading age [50], a reading speed score, a reading accuracy score, and a combined accuracy and speed index, called reading efficiency [51].
Significant texts
La Mouette, Le Pingouin (Evaléo 6–15© [52]): These two meaningful texts (n = 450 words) are equally balanced in terms of word and sentence length, lexical frequency, and syllabic and phonemic complexity to control a retest effect. Reading time and reading accuracy are measured. The maximum reading time is 2 min.
Two-minute word reading
Eval2M (Evaléo 6–15© [52]): This test (n = 263 words) assesses the percentage of words presented in 10 columns and ordered based on length and frequency correctly read within a limited time of 2 min.
Regular, irregular and pseudo-word reading
Evalec© [53]: This computerized test displays the words that need to be identified individually on the screen. The unique feature of this test is to measure the time needed to correctly read words using voice detection. The lexical or sublexical reading processes are assessed by calculating the latency time of correctly read items in msec and the error percentage when reading regular words (n = 36), pseudo-words (n = 36), and irregular words (n = 36).
Reading comprehension assessment
The ORLEC L3 test [5456] assesses word decoding speed and sentence comprehension. This test presents sentences that need to be finished (n = 36) with a word chosen from 5 suggested words. The raw score corresponds to the number of correct items completed in 5 min.
Spelling assessment
Chronosdictées© [57]: Two dictations of sentences ("A" and "B" for test and retest) are proposed to assess lexical, morphosyntactic and phonetic spelling for each grade of primary and secondary school. Results are given in number of phonetic, lexical, and grammatical errors and in number of segmentation errors and word omissions.

Underlying cognitive process assessment

Phonological processes

The software used for all these tasks calculates a speed and an accuracy score (Evalec© [53]).
Phonological short-term memory
The pseudo-word repetition task assesses the phonological short-term memory and is composed of pseudo-words of simple consonant/vowel (CV) syllabic structure (n = 12) and of pseudo-words of complex consonant/vowel/consonant (CCV) syllabic structure (n = 12), from 3 to 6 syllables.
Phonological analysis
The task of removing the first syllable from trisyllabic pseudowords (n = 10) assesses phonological analysis (e.g. coluti/luti). Two tasks of first phoneme removal from monosyllabic pseudo-words (n = 24) assess phonemic analysis (e.g. baf/af and tru/ru).
Rapid Automatized Naming
The color naming task assesses rapid automatized naming. Two formats are presented: a matrix of visual color (n = 54) and a matrix of written color names (n = 54) displayed in 9 lines of 6 colors in a random order. Three colors have a CVC syllabic structure in French (rouge, jaune, vert) and three colors have a CCV syllabic structure in French (bleu, blanc, gris).

Visual-attentional span

The visual-attentional span is measured by a global report task and a partial report task (Evadys© [58]). In the global report condition, the subject has to name a sequence of five consonants immediately after the sequence disappears from the screen. In the partial report condition, a vertical line appears and indicates the position of the letter to be named among the five letters displayed. The letter sequences are assembled to avoid activating any memorized lexical knowledge and to prevent any perceptual crowding. An isolated letter identification task is presented beforehand in order to exclude a letter recognition disorder. The software calculates a score in number of successful sequences and a letter span.

Global or local visual analysis

SIGL© software [59] assesses the ability to focus the attention on a global or on a local visual information analysis mode. The stimuli are hierarchized drawings displayed during 175 ms. The software calculates the gap of performance between the control condition and the interference condition in order to assess the local and global interference. Results are given in response times and in error percentages. To determine the interference asymmetry, the local interference effect is subtracted from the global interference effect.

Complementary assessments

Memory span

Digit span
Verbal memory is assessed by a repetition task of 2 to 7 numbers in forward (short-term memory) and in backward (working memory) order (Evaléo 6–15©, [52]). The digit span is determined by the number of correctly repeated numbers.
Visual-spatial span
The Corsi block-tapping test (CORSI© [60]) consists in reproducing the sequence in which the clinician points to different cubes, in the same or in reverse order. The number of cubes tapped in the sequence is progressively increased to determine the visual-spatial span.

Oral language assessment

If the oral language of the participant has not been assessed previously, three measures will be taken to assess lexical stock and morphosyntactic oral comprehension. The image naming task (Evaléo 6–15©, [52]) assesses the participant’s lexical stock of known words and the naming latency time. The image/word association (Evaléo 6–15©, [52]) assesses the comprehension’s lexical stock. The image/phrase association (E.CO.S.SE, [61]) assesses the syntactic-semantic comprehension.

Procedure

For this multiple-baseline A1BCA2 design across four participants, A1 phase corresponds to the baseline, where the participants follow a standard speech therapy remediation program. This standard remediation corresponds to a speech and language therapy intervention without daily training. The speech and language therapists target the symptoms and pathological behavioral manifestations of dyslexia without specifically working on the underlying cognitive processes. Both the B and C phases correspond to two distinct types of audio-phonological training, respectively categorical perception (CP) training, and rapid automatized naming (RAN) training. During both the B and C phases, each training session is coupled with phonemic analysis (PA) training and a reading and writing task. A2 phase represents the post intervention phase where the participants stop the intensive training but continue to follow a standard remediation program. The detailed timeline of the participants is presented in Fig. 1. To obtain enough data for statistical analysis, each phase of this protocol involves at least 6 measurement sessions [62] held on a weekly basis. Phases B and C will be introduced in a staggered timeframe across participants. In other words, each participant's baseline will vary in length and number of measurement points. Thus, with one measurement session per week, Participants 1, 2, 3 and 4 will respectively begin the intervention after a baseline period of 6 weeks (6 measurement sessions), 7 weeks (7 measurement sessions), 8 weeks (8 measurement sessions), and 9 weeks (9 measurement sessions). The two training phases are designed to avoid any confounding values of time or number of sessions. The follow-up phase (A2) will also be introduced sequentially but will last 6 weeks (6 measures) for all participants. As a result, the total duration of the four phases will not exceed 30 weeks. Throughout the entire protocol, patients will continue standard weekly speech therapy sessions with their speech and language therapist. Measurement sessions will be performed at the end of each of these sessions by the principal investigator.

Objectives

The main objective is to compare the effect on the phonological abilities of HIP-DD subjects of an intensive intervention combining training in categorical perception (CP), rapid automatized naming (RAN), and phonemic analysis (PA) versus a standard remediation program, at the end of the training program and at 6 weeks post-training.
Main Hypothesis: We assume that intensive training coupled with standard dyslexia remediation will significantly improve participants' phonological skills.
Two secondary objectives were identified:
  • Objective 2: To compare the effect on reading efficiency of an intensive intervention on CP, RAN, and PA versus standard remediation, at the end of the training program and at 6 weeks post-training.
  • Hypothesis 2: We hypothesize that intensive CP, RAN, and PA training significantly improves reading skills at the end of the training period and 6 weeks after.
  • Objective 3: We explore the effect of the sequence of CP and RAN training phases on participant’s phonological and reading skills.

Intervention

Remediation protocol

In phase A, the standard speech therapy session for dyslexia contains grapheme-phoneme conversion exercises, lexical spelling tasks, and phonological awareness tasks without visual support. According to the NGAP [63], the session should last at least 30 min.
During the B and C intervention phases, the children continue standard speech therapy sessions (as in phase A) and add 15 min of daily training at home, 5 days a week. The main experimenter organizes a practice training session with the legal guardians and patients at the beginning of the protocol to ensure that the participants understand how to complete the training sessions correctly without any assistance. The first type of intervention corresponds to a 10-min training session on categorical perception using Rapdys© [43]. The second type of intervention is rapid automatized naming and lasts 10 min. Both interventions (RAN and CP) are systematically combined with 5 additional minutes of phonemic analysis. On the two days off each week, the child does not perform any training.

Descriptions of specific interventions

Categorial perception—rapdys©
Categorical perception training is carried out at home: 10 min per day, 5 times per week for the whole duration of the phase using RAPDYS© [43]. This software proposes a series of training sessions allowing the patient to discriminate more and more finely between two phonemes with different voicing (e.g. /d/ and /t/). The stimuli used depend on the level of difficulty: 5 levels based on the difference in VOT (Voice Onset Time) between the stimuli. Training consists of two tasks: identification and discrimination. In the identification task, the participant listens to a sound stimulus and has to determine which phoneme was heard. In the discrimination task, the participant hears two phonemes in a row and has to say whether they were the same or different.
Rapid automatized naming—naming speed
The rapid automatized RAN training is performed at home: 10 min per day, 5 days per week for the whole duration of the phase, using the Naming Speed program. This program was created by Karine Harrar Eskinazi, Julie Nothelier, and Marine Versio for the needs of the forthcoming study "Developmental dyslexia and method of remediation (DDMR): Multimodal intervention in French children aged from 8 to 13 years'' [45] because no other software for rapid naming training was available in French. This program was inspired by the Italian software "Run the RAN" [42]. Five black and white drawings of objects from the LEAD lexicon database [64] are displayed on the screen and repeated on horizontal lines randomly displayed in boards of 20 to 60 stimuli. The patient must name, as quickly and accurately as possible, all the images presented, from left to right (in the direction of reading) following an imposed cadence; a red frame is automatically displayed on the screen and gives the patient the naming rhythm. A single image is framed at first, and as the training progresses, the frame surrounds more images (up to 5) and the naming speed increases (from 200 ms/item to 50 ms/item) (Fig. 2).
Phonological analysis—phoneme fusion and segmentation (in both the b and c phases)
During both training phases (PC and RAN), a phonemic analysis task is associated with the daily exercises. This task combines two processes: phonemic segmentation and phonemic fusion. Phonemic fusion consists in merging phonemes pronounced in oral form; for instance, the child hears the phonemes /p/-/i/-/r/-/õ/ one after the other and has to fuse the phonemes together in order to pronounce the logatome "piron". The segmentation task is the exact opposite: the child hears the logatome and must segment it into phonemes. In order to train the grapheme-phoneme conversion processes, the child is asked to read the ten items at the end of the training session, and to train the phoneme-grapheme conversion process, he must also write them. The logatomes are computer-generated. Participants are asked to fuse ten logatomes per day after each categorical perception training and to segment ten logatomes per day after each RAN training.

Outcomes

The two primary judgment criteria are each evaluated by three different measures. In order to meet the internal and external validity criteria of the Risk of Bias in N of 1 Trials (RoBiNT) scale [65], the outcome measures are assessed once a week throughout the entire protocol, from the beginning of phase A1 to the end of phase A2. The measurement session is performed at the end of each weekly speech and language therapy appointment (Fig. 3).

Primary outcomes

The first outcome is a performance gain in audio-phonological skills. It is evaluated by three distinct measures as recommended by Tate et al. [66]:
Rapdys© Assessment
Rapdys© [43] is a program designed to assess and improve the discrimination of voicing boundaries of the phonemes of the French language (opposition of muted and voiced consonant sounds, for example /b/ & /d/). The integrated evaluation software provides an assessment of the child's perceptual system and thus an objective measure of potential progress achieved during training. According to the authors, there is no test/retest effect since the stimuli are presented in a random order and no feedback is provided. Studies conducted did not show any increase in perceptual performance in the control group when the evaluation task was repeated at regular intervals. The score is the percentage of correct answers and will be the judgment criterion for this measure.
Phoneme counting
To assess phonological analysis skills, a list of 10 logatomes is presented to patients from an oral input. The examiner reads the logatome to the child and asks for a count of the phonemes composing it. For example, for the logatome /pabou/, the participant has to segment the phonemes and count them: /p/-/a/-/b/-/ou/ = four phonemes. The child has to answer four. The logatome lists were generated from the free software Logatron [58]. In order to obtain 10 different logatomes in each of the 30 repeated measurement sessions, a list of 300 bisyllabic logatomes was generated. The logatomes in this list follow the phonotactic rules of French. Some of them contain complex phoneme groups (consonant clusters) and others are simpler. If the child makes a mistake, the item is presented again until it is successfully answered. The time (in seconds) is measured and used as a judgement criterion.
Ran colors
To measure rapid automatized naming skills, a board of 54 colors (9 lines of 6 colors) is presented in a random order. Its organization is based on the initial “Naming Speed” board [45] but with more complex colors (purple, orange, turquoise etc.) displayed in a different computer-generated random order for each measurement to reduce the learning effect. The participant must name all the colors on the board in the direction of reading (from left to right) and as quickly as possible while making as few mistakes as possible. The raw scores collected are the time in seconds and the number of errors per board. An accuracy score is calculated by dividing the number of correctly named colors by the total number of colors on a board. A second score is computed by dividing the time spent naming the entire board by the number of correctly named colors. An example of a test board is shown in Fig. 4 next to the preliminary practice line.

Secondary outcomes

The generalization measure –the measure of reading efficiency– is also assessed using three different scores, all from the "DeltaText" leximetry test (Bedoin, 2017).
The DeltaText consists of four meaningless texts matched for word count and syntactic, lexical, and phonological difficulty. Each text is composed of 201 regular words. The maximum reading time is set to 3 min. During the test, the number of errors and the number of words read at the end of 1 mn 30 and at the end of the 3 min are recorded. The instruction given to the patient is to read as quickly as possible and with the fewest mistakes as possible. If the 201 words are read before the end of the allocated time, the reading time is recorded. Three measures are obtained:
Reading speed
The reading speed (in number of words read per minute).
Reading accuracy
The reading accuracy (in percentage of words correctly read).
Reading efficiency
The reading efficiency: CTL = [C (number of words read correctly) / TL (child reading time)] x 120 s (maximum reading time)].

Control item

To control the specificity of the training, a control test is also proposed, in which non-linguistic target symbols must be circled among visual distractors. A board contains 300 items, including 30 targets. The participant has to find all the targets among the 270 distractor animals on the board during a given time. The board is printed in A4 format, and the participant has 60 s to circle as many targets as possible as fast as possible without circling the distractors. For each measurement session, a new board is randomly generated in a different disposition through a computerized process. An example of a board is shown in Fig. 5.

Randomization allocation and blinding

To introduce a single-blind effect, the children will know what their training program involves but not the expected effects. To ensure double blinding, the speech therapist will be aware of the experimental phases the children are in, but will not intervene in the measurements nor in the collection of results. The members of the experimental teams will not know which phase the children are in, and will simply collect the measurements during the weekly measurement session. This will maintain double blinding.
The internal validity of the single-case experimental design (SCED) is increased when patients are assigned in an unbiased manner to the baseline duration [67]. Thus, patients will be randomly assigned to the different baseline lengths using a simple computerized randomization procedure. The duration of the intervention phases is also determined in a counterbalanced manner. Two of the four participants will begin with categorical perception training and the other two will begin with rapid automatized naming training. This order follows the baseline durations assigned at the beginning of the experiment: the number of weeks per phase is calculated to avoid confounding variables. The allocation of participants to the 4 designs (type of intervention and length) is thus randomized.

Training supervision and treatment integrity

The daily training sessions are carried out at the patient's home, and the parents or legal guardians are asked to fill in protocol monitoring forms. These forms contain both simplified training instructions for the parents and charts to record the child's scores each day. However, the weekly speech and language therapy sessions also provide an effective way to ensure proper completion of the protocol and compliance with the training instructions. Therefore, these records will be checked at each weekly speech and language therapy appointment to confirm that the training program is correctly followed. The RapDys© software will also provide a record of the dates and results of each training session. The measurements will be assessed by a member of experimental teams after each weekly speech therapy session.

Confidentiality

All computerized experimental data from the study will be stored on a password secured network only known by the investigators. Paper data will be kept in a locked case. Patients will be anonymized when data are published.

Refusal of study participation and drop-out

Consent for study participation is obtained at the pre-inclusion assessment and no justification is required in case of refusal. Withdrawal from the protocol is possible at any time at the request of the patient or legal guardian and in case of non-compliance. The criteria for exceptional discontinuation or modification of the protocol are withdrawal of consent to participate, a medical condition, or any other event involving a non-compliance.

Adverse events

There are no predicted adverse or dangerous events for the participants in this study.

Intended descriptive and inferential statistical analyses

First, a visual and descriptive analysis will be performed (Glass’∆ and Cohen’s d [68]). Then, we will use the Tau-U analysis [69], which measures effect size for single-case studies. The baseline trend could be controlled by the Baseline Corrected Tau [70].
To fit the design, missing measurement sessions will be considered as missing data and will not be imputed. Also, according to James E. Pustejovsky, we will make available the raw data used for effect size calculations, so that other researchers can easily replicate and extend our analyses [71].

Discussion

This paper presents an experimental protocol for a single-case multiple-baseline design across 4 participants to observe the effects of an intensive audio-phonological training program on the phonological and reading skills of four children with developmental dyslexia and HIP (HIP-DD). The lack of data regarding the remediation of HIP-DD children forces clinicians to use non-specific remediation methods for twice-exceptional children, which are often not very effective.
The results of this study, if successful, will provide a partial answer to this problem. Furthermore, the link between the single-case design and Evidence Based Practice (EBP) [72], which is widely used by clinicians in their daily practice, opens a real gateway between the research and clinical worlds. The American Speech-Language-Hearing Association has also recommended the use of EBP in interventions for communication disorders since 2005 [73]. Most speech and language therapists are therefore familiar with the use of baselines and with the within-subject control approach. Thus, if the results are consistent, they can be directly interpreted by clinicians, and the data regarding remediation can be easily adapted and quickly applied in their practice.
Furthermore, small sample studies allow for a detailed qualitative analysis and are a good alternative to group studies when the target population is rare. The single-case design allows a more exhaustive qualitative analysis of the anamnesis and screening data in order to better understand the cognitive language profile of each participant. The staggered configuration of the participants' timeline allows a control of temporal and session number variables. Despite the thorough attention paid to respecting scientific criteria that ensure the highest reliability of the outcomes, some limitations were found when preparing this protocol. To avoid cognitive overload and participant dropout, an intervention targeting all underlying cognitive deficits was excluded. First, the time required for a measurement session would be excessive, as would the total duration of the protocol. Also, since phonological theory is still the most widely believed causal hypothesis of developmental dyslexia [66], an intervention that only targets the audio-phonological processes involved in dyslexia was selected. Thus, only RAN and CP were measured and offered as training in this study. A similar protocol focusing on visual-attentional aspects could be considered at a later stage. If the results are conclusive, a remediation protocol focusing on all underlying cognitive deficits should be tested in a larger sample study without the constraints of repeated measurements induced by the SCED.
In conclusion, the population of HIP-DD patients represents a significant part of speech and language therapists' patient base but remains underrepresented in the scientific literature. Consequently, the therapeutic approaches for these patients are still not sufficiently structured and validated. The results of this study should constitute a starting point for further progress in the remediation of written language disorders in children with HIP, both in the clinical and the research fields.

Acknowledgements

Trial status

Recruitment for this study has not started yet and is expected to occur in January 2023. Version identifier is no. ID RCB 2019-A01453-54, 19-HPNCL-02, 07/18/2019. Developmental Dyslexia and Remediation Methods—Full Text View—ClinicalTrials.gov

Declarations

This study received ethics approval from two independent expert committees. First ethics approval was provided by the French National Committee for the Protection of Persons (CPP); Ile de France IV, IRB 00003835, reference number 2019/50, ID-RCB 2019-A01453-54. Second ethics approval was provided by the local Ethics Committee for Non-Interventional Research at Université Côte d'Azur (CERNI); reference number 2019–7.
Written informed consent will be received from each participant’s legal guardian or parent in accordance with the Jardé law decree on research involving the human person (Decree no. 2016–1537 of 16 November 2016) and the Declaration of Helsinki. Major modifications to the protocol will be submitted to the CPP IDF VI ethics committee for approval. Any changes will be implemented and added to the protocol. The modifications will be communicated in a new consent form to the study participants. The results of the study will be shared with patients and legal guardians upon request.
Not applicable—No individual data were shared for the purpose of this article.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interest.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Delaubier J-P. La scolarisation des élèves intellectuellement précoces : rapport à Monsieur le Ministre de l’éducation nationale. Rapp Off Ministère L’Éducation Natl. 2002. Delaubier J-P. La scolarisation des élèves intellectuellement précoces : rapport à Monsieur le Ministre de l’éducation nationale. Rapp Off Ministère L’Éducation Natl. 2002.
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Toffalini E, Pezzuti L, Cornoldi C. Einstein and dyslexia: Is giftedness more frequent in children with a specific learning disorder than in typically developing children? Intelligence. 2017;62:175–9.CrossRef Toffalini E, Pezzuti L, Cornoldi C. Einstein and dyslexia: Is giftedness more frequent in children with a specific learning disorder than in typically developing children? Intelligence. 2017;62:175–9.CrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Pradeille F-M. The support of the family by the general practitioner, since the discovery of giftness for children with undera-chievement to the establishment of solutions. U - Thesis (Thesis). Université de Picardie; 2012. Pradeille F-M. The support of the family by the general practitioner, since the discovery of giftness for children with undera-chievement to the establishment of solutions. U - Thesis (Thesis). Université de Picardie; 2012.
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Winner E. Surdoués: mythes et réalités. Aubier; 1997. Winner E. Surdoués: mythes et réalités. Aubier; 1997.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Turon-Lagot E. WISC-IV : Une mesure des manifestations de l’intelligence chez l’enfant. Eric Turon-Lagot; 2012. Turon-Lagot E. WISC-IV : Une mesure des manifestations de l’intelligence chez l’enfant. Eric Turon-Lagot; 2012.
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Revol O, Bléandonu G. Enfants intellectuellement précoces : comment les dépister ? Arch Pédiatrie. 2012;19:340–3.CrossRef Revol O, Bléandonu G. Enfants intellectuellement précoces : comment les dépister ? Arch Pédiatrie. 2012;19:340–3.CrossRef
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Wahl G. Les enfants intellectuellement précoces. Paris cedex 14: Presses Universitaires de France; 2015. Wahl G. Les enfants intellectuellement précoces. Paris cedex 14: Presses Universitaires de France; 2015.
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Vaumoron S, Pouhet A. Tests de QI : et que faire après ? Editions Tom Pousse; 2019. Vaumoron S, Pouhet A. Tests de QI : et que faire après ? Editions Tom Pousse; 2019.
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Grégoire J. Les défis actuels de l’identification des enfants à haut potentiel. ANAE Approche Neuropsychol Apprentiss Chez Enfant. 2012;25. Grégoire J. Les défis actuels de l’identification des enfants à haut potentiel. ANAE Approche Neuropsychol Apprentiss Chez Enfant. 2012;25.
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Liratni M, Wagner A, Pry R. Performances d’écriture de 12 enfants à haut potentiel intellectuel. ANAE Approche Neuropsychol Apprentiss Chez Enfant. 2012;116:86–94. Liratni M, Wagner A, Pry R. Performances d’écriture de 12 enfants à haut potentiel intellectuel. ANAE Approche Neuropsychol Apprentiss Chez Enfant. 2012;116:86–94.
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Suprano I, Delon-Martin C, Kocevar G, Stamile C, Hannoun S, Achard S, et al. Topological modification of brain networks organization in children with high intelligence quotient: a resting-state fMRI study. Front Hum Neurosci. 2019;13:241. Suprano I, Delon-Martin C, Kocevar G, Stamile C, Hannoun S, Achard S, et al. Topological modification of brain networks organization in children with high intelligence quotient: a resting-state fMRI study. Front Hum Neurosci. 2019;13:241.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Solé-Casals J, Serra-Grabulosa JM, Romero-Garcia R, Vilaseca G, Adan A, Vilaró N, et al. Structural brain network of gifted children has a more integrated and versatile topology. Brain Struct Funct. 2019;224:2373–83.CrossRefPubMed Solé-Casals J, Serra-Grabulosa JM, Romero-Garcia R, Vilaseca G, Adan A, Vilaró N, et al. Structural brain network of gifted children has a more integrated and versatile topology. Brain Struct Funct. 2019;224:2373–83.CrossRefPubMed
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Jin S-H, Kwon Y-J, Jeong J-S, Kwon S-W, Shin D-H. Differences in Brain Information Transmission between Gifted and Normal Children during Scientific Hypothesis Generation. Brain Cogn. 2006;62:191–7.CrossRefPubMed Jin S-H, Kwon Y-J, Jeong J-S, Kwon S-W, Shin D-H. Differences in Brain Information Transmission between Gifted and Normal Children during Scientific Hypothesis Generation. Brain Cogn. 2006;62:191–7.CrossRefPubMed
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Lee KH, Choi YY, Gray JR, Cho SH, Chae J-H, Lee S, et al. Neural correlates of superior intelligence: stronger recruitment of posterior parietal cortex. Neuroimage. 2006;29:578–86.CrossRefPubMed Lee KH, Choi YY, Gray JR, Cho SH, Chae J-H, Lee S, et al. Neural correlates of superior intelligence: stronger recruitment of posterior parietal cortex. Neuroimage. 2006;29:578–86.CrossRefPubMed
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Fraenz C, Schlüter C, Friedrich P, Jung RE, Güntürkün O, Genç E. Interindividual differences in matrix reasoning are linked to functional connectivity between brain regions nominated by Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory. Intelligence. 2021;87:101545.CrossRef Fraenz C, Schlüter C, Friedrich P, Jung RE, Güntürkün O, Genç E. Interindividual differences in matrix reasoning are linked to functional connectivity between brain regions nominated by Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory. Intelligence. 2021;87:101545.CrossRef
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Luders E, Narr KL, Bilder RM, Thompson PM, Szeszko PR, Hamilton L, et al. Positive correlations between corpus callosum thickness and intelligence. Neuroimage. 2007;37:1457–64.CrossRefPubMed Luders E, Narr KL, Bilder RM, Thompson PM, Szeszko PR, Hamilton L, et al. Positive correlations between corpus callosum thickness and intelligence. Neuroimage. 2007;37:1457–64.CrossRefPubMed
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Gignac GE, Bates TC. Brain volume and intelligence: The moderating role of intelligence measurement quality. Intelligence. 2017;64:18–29.CrossRef Gignac GE, Bates TC. Brain volume and intelligence: The moderating role of intelligence measurement quality. Intelligence. 2017;64:18–29.CrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Fifth. American Psychiatric Association; 2013. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Fifth. American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Lachmann T, van Leeuwen C. Reading as functional coordination: not recycling but a novel synthesis. Front Psychol. 2014;5:1046. Lachmann T, van Leeuwen C. Reading as functional coordination: not recycling but a novel synthesis. Front Psychol. 2014;5:1046.
20.
Zurück zum Zitat Saksida A, Iannuzzi S, Bogliotti C, Chaix Y, Démonet J-F, Bricout L. Phonological skills, visual attention span, and visual stress in developmental dyslexia. Dev Psychol. 2016;52:1503–16.CrossRefPubMed Saksida A, Iannuzzi S, Bogliotti C, Chaix Y, Démonet J-F, Bricout L. Phonological skills, visual attention span, and visual stress in developmental dyslexia. Dev Psychol. 2016;52:1503–16.CrossRefPubMed
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Menghini D, Carlesimo GA, Marotta L, Finzi A, Vicari S. Developmental dyslexia and explicit long-term memory. Dyslexia. 2010;16:213–25.CrossRefPubMed Menghini D, Carlesimo GA, Marotta L, Finzi A, Vicari S. Developmental dyslexia and explicit long-term memory. Dyslexia. 2010;16:213–25.CrossRefPubMed
22.
Zurück zum Zitat Pennington BF. From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition. 2006;101:385–413.CrossRefPubMed Pennington BF. From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition. 2006;101:385–413.CrossRefPubMed
23.
Zurück zum Zitat van Bergen E, van der Leij A, de Jong PF. The intergenerational multiple deficit model and the case of dyslexia. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:346. van Bergen E, van der Leij A, de Jong PF. The intergenerational multiple deficit model and the case of dyslexia. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:346.
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Zoubrinetzky R, Bielle F, Valdois S. New Insights on Developmental Dyslexia Subtypes: Heterogeneity of Mixed Reading Profiles. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e99337.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral Zoubrinetzky R, Bielle F, Valdois S. New Insights on Developmental Dyslexia Subtypes: Heterogeneity of Mixed Reading Profiles. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e99337.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Peters JL, De Losa L, Bavin EL, Crewther SG. Efficacy of dynamic visuo-attentional interventions for reading in dyslexic and neurotypical children: A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;100:58–76.CrossRefPubMed Peters JL, De Losa L, Bavin EL, Crewther SG. Efficacy of dynamic visuo-attentional interventions for reading in dyslexic and neurotypical children: A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2019;100:58–76.CrossRefPubMed
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Ziegler J, Perry C, Zorzi M. Modeling the Variability of Developmental Dyslexia. 2019. Ziegler J, Perry C, Zorzi M. Modeling the Variability of Developmental Dyslexia. 2019.
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Gombert J-E. Le Développement métalinguistique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France - PUF; 1990. Gombert J-E. Le Développement métalinguistique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France - PUF; 1990.
28.
Zurück zum Zitat Swan D, Goswami U. Picture Naming Deficits in Developmental Dyslexia: The Phonological Representations Hypothesis. Brain Lang. 1997;56:334–53.CrossRefPubMed Swan D, Goswami U. Picture Naming Deficits in Developmental Dyslexia: The Phonological Representations Hypothesis. Brain Lang. 1997;56:334–53.CrossRefPubMed
29.
Zurück zum Zitat Wolf M, Bowers PG. Naming-Speed Processes and Developmental Reading Disabilities: An Introduction to the Special Issue on the Double-Deficit Hypothesis. J Learn Disabil. 2000;33:322–4.CrossRefPubMed Wolf M, Bowers PG. Naming-Speed Processes and Developmental Reading Disabilities: An Introduction to the Special Issue on the Double-Deficit Hypothesis. J Learn Disabil. 2000;33:322–4.CrossRefPubMed
30.
Zurück zum Zitat Mundy IR, Hannant P. Exploring the phonological profiles of children with reading difficulties: A multiple case study. Dyslexia. 2020;26:411–26.CrossRefPubMed Mundy IR, Hannant P. Exploring the phonological profiles of children with reading difficulties: A multiple case study. Dyslexia. 2020;26:411–26.CrossRefPubMed
31.
Zurück zum Zitat Zoubrinetzky R, Collet G, Serniclaes W, Nguyen-Morel M-A, Valdois S. Relationships between categorical perception of phonemes, phoneme awareness, and visual attention span in developmental dyslexia. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0151015. Zoubrinetzky R, Collet G, Serniclaes W, Nguyen-Morel M-A, Valdois S. Relationships between categorical perception of phonemes, phoneme awareness, and visual attention span in developmental dyslexia. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0151015.
32.
Zurück zum Zitat Serniclaes W, Van Heghe S, Mousty P, Carré R, Sprenger-Charolles L. Allophonic mode of speech perception in dyslexia. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004;87:336–61.CrossRefPubMed Serniclaes W, Van Heghe S, Mousty P, Carré R, Sprenger-Charolles L. Allophonic mode of speech perception in dyslexia. J Exp Child Psychol. 2004;87:336–61.CrossRefPubMed
33.
Zurück zum Zitat Brody LE, Mills CJ. Gifted Children with Learning Disabilities: A Review of the Issues. J Learn Disabil. 1997;30:282–96.CrossRefPubMed Brody LE, Mills CJ. Gifted Children with Learning Disabilities: A Review of the Issues. J Learn Disabil. 1997;30:282–96.CrossRefPubMed
34.
Zurück zum Zitat van Viersen S, de Bree EH, Kroesbergen EH, Slot EM, de Jong PF. Risk and protective factors in gifted children with dyslexia. Ann Dyslexia. 2015;65:178–98.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral van Viersen S, de Bree EH, Kroesbergen EH, Slot EM, de Jong PF. Risk and protective factors in gifted children with dyslexia. Ann Dyslexia. 2015;65:178–98.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentral
35.
Zurück zum Zitat van Viersen S, Kroesbergen EH, Slot EM, de Bree EH. High Reading Skills Mask Dyslexia in Gifted Children. J Learn Disabil. 2014;49:189–99.CrossRefPubMed van Viersen S, Kroesbergen EH, Slot EM, de Bree EH. High Reading Skills Mask Dyslexia in Gifted Children. J Learn Disabil. 2014;49:189–99.CrossRefPubMed
36.
Zurück zum Zitat Berninger VW, Abbott RD. Differences Between Children With Dyslexia Who Are and Are Not Gifted in Verbal Reasoning. Gift Child Q. 2013;57:223–33.CrossRef Berninger VW, Abbott RD. Differences Between Children With Dyslexia Who Are and Are Not Gifted in Verbal Reasoning. Gift Child Q. 2013;57:223–33.CrossRef
37.
Zurück zum Zitat Foley Nicpon M, Allmon A, Sieck B, Stinson RD. Empirical Investigation of Twice-Exceptionality: Where Have We Been and Where Are We Going? Gift Child Q. 2011;55:3–17.CrossRef Foley Nicpon M, Allmon A, Sieck B, Stinson RD. Empirical Investigation of Twice-Exceptionality: Where Have We Been and Where Are We Going? Gift Child Q. 2011;55:3–17.CrossRef
38.
Zurück zum Zitat Assouline SG, Foley Nicpon M, Whiteman C. Cognitive and Psychosocial Characteristics of Gifted Students With Written Language Disability. Gift Child Q. 2010;54:102–15.CrossRef Assouline SG, Foley Nicpon M, Whiteman C. Cognitive and Psychosocial Characteristics of Gifted Students With Written Language Disability. Gift Child Q. 2010;54:102–15.CrossRef
39.
Zurück zum Zitat Vander Stappen C, Reybroeck MV. Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming are independent phonological competencies with specific impacts on word reading and spelling: an intervention study. Front Psychol. 2018;9:320. Vander Stappen C, Reybroeck MV. Phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming are independent phonological competencies with specific impacts on word reading and spelling: an intervention study. Front Psychol. 2018;9:320.
40.
Zurück zum Zitat Hatcher PJ, Hulme C, Snowling MJ. Explicit phoneme training combined with phonic reading instruction helps young children at risk of reading failure. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45:338–58.CrossRefPubMed Hatcher PJ, Hulme C, Snowling MJ. Explicit phoneme training combined with phonic reading instruction helps young children at risk of reading failure. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45:338–58.CrossRefPubMed
41.
Zurück zum Zitat Layes S, Guendouz M, Lalonde R, Rebai M. Combined Phonological Awareness and Print Knowledge Training Improves Reading Accuracy and Comprehension in Children with Reading Disabilities. Int J Disabil Dev Educ. 2020;0:1–15. Layes S, Guendouz M, Lalonde R, Rebai M. Combined Phonological Awareness and Print Knowledge Training Improves Reading Accuracy and Comprehension in Children with Reading Disabilities. Int J Disabil Dev Educ. 2020;0:1–15.
42.
Zurück zum Zitat Pecini C, Spoglianti S, Bonetti S, Lieto MCD, Guaran F, Martinelli A, et al. Training RAN or reading? A telerehabilitation study on developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2019;25:318–31.CrossRefPubMed Pecini C, Spoglianti S, Bonetti S, Lieto MCD, Guaran F, Martinelli A, et al. Training RAN or reading? A telerehabilitation study on developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia. 2019;25:318–31.CrossRefPubMed
43.
Zurück zum Zitat Serniclaes W, Gregory C, Cécile C. Remédiation audio-phonologique de la dyslexie (RapDys©) : un logiciel visant à modifier la perception allophonique des sons de la parole en perception phonémique. ANAE - Approche Neuropsychol Apprentiss Chez Enfant. 2017;148:257–3. Serniclaes W, Gregory C, Cécile C. Remédiation audio-phonologique de la dyslexie (RapDys©) : un logiciel visant à modifier la perception allophonique des sons de la parole en perception phonémique. ANAE - Approche Neuropsychol Apprentiss Chez Enfant. 2017;148:257–3.
44.
Zurück zum Zitat Ronksley-Pavia M. A Model of Twice-Exceptionality: Explaining and Defining the Apparent Paradoxical Combination of Disability and Giftedness in Childhood. J Educ Gift. 2015;38:318–40.CrossRef Ronksley-Pavia M. A Model of Twice-Exceptionality: Explaining and Defining the Apparent Paradoxical Combination of Disability and Giftedness in Childhood. J Educ Gift. 2015;38:318–40.CrossRef
45.
Zurück zum Zitat Harrar-Eskinazi KL, De Cara B, Leloup G, Nothelier J, Caci H, Ziegler JC, et al. Multimodal intervention in 8- to 13-year-old French dyslexic readers: Study protocol for a randomized multicenter controlled crossover trial. BMC Pediatr. 2022;22:741. Harrar-Eskinazi KL, De Cara B, Leloup G, Nothelier J, Caci H, Ziegler JC, et al. Multimodal intervention in 8- to 13-year-old French dyslexic readers: Study protocol for a randomized multicenter controlled crossover trial. BMC Pediatr. 2022;22:741.
46.
Zurück zum Zitat Ledford JR, Wolery M. Procedural Fidelity: An Analysis of Measurement and Reporting Practices. J Early Interv. 2013;35:173–93.CrossRef Ledford JR, Wolery M. Procedural Fidelity: An Analysis of Measurement and Reporting Practices. J Early Interv. 2013;35:173–93.CrossRef
47.
Zurück zum Zitat Gana K, Gallé-Tessonneau M, Broc G. Le protocole individuel en psychologie : tutoriel à l’usage des psychologues praticiens. Prat Psychol. 2019;25:153–67. Gana K, Gallé-Tessonneau M, Broc G. Le protocole individuel en psychologie : tutoriel à l’usage des psychologues praticiens. Prat Psychol. 2019;25:153–67.
49.
Zurück zum Zitat Janosky JE, Leininger SL, Hoerger MP, Libkuman TM. Application of the Single Subject Design in Biomedicine. In: Janosky JE, Leininger SL, Hoerger MP, Libkuman TM, editors. Single Subject Designs in Biomedicine. Dordrecht: Springer, Netherlands; 2009. p. 81–96.CrossRef Janosky JE, Leininger SL, Hoerger MP, Libkuman TM. Application of the Single Subject Design in Biomedicine. In: Janosky JE, Leininger SL, Hoerger MP, Libkuman TM, editors. Single Subject Designs in Biomedicine. Dordrecht: Springer, Netherlands; 2009. p. 81–96.CrossRef
50.
Zurück zum Zitat Lefavrais P. Manuel du test de l’alouette. Paris, France: Centre de psychologie appliquée; 1967. Lefavrais P. Manuel du test de l’alouette. Paris, France: Centre de psychologie appliquée; 1967.
51.
Zurück zum Zitat Lefavrais P. Alouette-R: test d’analyse de la vitesse en lecture à partir d’un texte. Paris: Les éditions du centre de psychologie appliquée; 2005. Lefavrais P. Alouette-R: test d’analyse de la vitesse en lecture à partir d’un texte. Paris: Les éditions du centre de psychologie appliquée; 2005.
52.
Zurück zum Zitat Launay L, Roustit J, Maeder C, Touzin M. Evaléo 6–15 : Evaluation du langage écrit et du langage oral 6–15 ans. Isbergues: Ortho édition; 2018. Launay L, Roustit J, Maeder C, Touzin M. Evaléo 6–15 : Evaluation du langage écrit et du langage oral 6–15 ans. Isbergues: Ortho édition; 2018.
53.
Zurück zum Zitat Sprenger-Charolles L, Colé P, Piquard-Kipffer A, EVALEC LG. Batterie informatisée d’évaluation diagnostique des troubles spécifiques d’apprentissage de la lecture. 2010. Sprenger-Charolles L, Colé P, Piquard-Kipffer A, EVALEC LG. Batterie informatisée d’évaluation diagnostique des troubles spécifiques d’apprentissage de la lecture. 2010.
54.
Zurück zum Zitat Lobrot M. OrLec : Batterie d’épreuves pour mesurer la lecture et l’ orthographe : partie lecture, partie orthographe. Etablissements D’Applications Psychotech EAP. 1988. Lobrot M. OrLec : Batterie d’épreuves pour mesurer la lecture et l’ orthographe : partie lecture, partie orthographe. Etablissements D’Applications Psychotech EAP. 1988.
55.
Zurück zum Zitat Lobrot M. OrLec : Batterie d’épreuves pour mesurer la lecture et l’ orthographe : partie lecture, partie orthographe. Beaumont-sur-Oise: Bureau d’études et de recherches; 1967. Lobrot M. OrLec : Batterie d’épreuves pour mesurer la lecture et l’ orthographe : partie lecture, partie orthographe. Beaumont-sur-Oise: Bureau d’études et de recherches; 1967.
56.
Zurück zum Zitat Pierart B, Gregoire J. Déchiffrer et comprendre : le test de closure en lecture revisité : Etalonnage belge du L3 de Lobrot. Déchiffrer Compr Test Clos En Lect Revisité Etalonnage Belge L3 Lobrot. 2004;39:87–100. Pierart B, Gregoire J. Déchiffrer et comprendre : le test de closure en lecture revisité : Etalonnage belge du L3 de Lobrot. Déchiffrer Compr Test Clos En Lect Revisité Etalonnage Belge L3 Lobrot. 2004;39:87–100.
57.
Zurück zum Zitat Baneath B, Boutard C, Alberti C. Chronosdictées : Outils d’évaluation des performances orthographiques : avec et sans contrainte temporelle : du CE1 à la troisième. Isbergues: Ortho édition; 2006. Baneath B, Boutard C, Alberti C. Chronosdictées : Outils d’évaluation des performances orthographiques : avec et sans contrainte temporelle : du CE1 à la troisième. Isbergues: Ortho édition; 2006.
58.
Zurück zum Zitat Valdois S, Guinet E, Embs JL, Bosse M-L, Peyrin C. Evadys : logiciel d’évaluation de l’empan visuo-attentionnel chez l’enfant dyslexique ; Coreva : Exercices progressifs d’entrainement des capacités d’empan visuo-attentionnel. ZA Proxima: Happy Neuron Pro; 2014. Valdois S, Guinet E, Embs JL, Bosse M-L, Peyrin C. Evadys : logiciel d’évaluation de l’empan visuo-attentionnel chez l’enfant dyslexique ; Coreva : Exercices progressifs d’entrainement des capacités d’empan visuo-attentionnel. ZA Proxima: Happy Neuron Pro; 2014.
59.
Zurück zum Zitat SIGL : Sélection et inhibition des traitements global et local. GNOSIA; 2014. SIGL : Sélection et inhibition des traitements global et local. GNOSIA; 2014.
60.
Zurück zum Zitat Kessels RPC, van ZMJE, Postma A, Kappelle LJ, de HEHF. The Corsi Block-Tapping Task: Standardization and Normative Data. Appl Neuropsychol. 2000;7:252–8. Kessels RPC, van ZMJE, Postma A, Kappelle LJ, de HEHF. The Corsi Block-Tapping Task: Standardization and Normative Data. Appl Neuropsychol. 2000;7:252–8.
61.
Zurück zum Zitat Lecocq P. É.co.s.se une épreuve de compréhension syntaxico-sémantique (manuel et épreuve): Deux volumes. Presses Univ. Septentrion; 1998. Lecocq P. É.co.s.se une épreuve de compréhension syntaxico-sémantique (manuel et épreuve): Deux volumes. Presses Univ. Septentrion; 1998.
62.
Zurück zum Zitat Krasny-Pacini A, Evans J. Single-case experimental designs to assess intervention effectiveness in rehabilitation: A practical guide. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2018;61:164–79.CrossRefPubMed Krasny-Pacini A, Evans J. Single-case experimental designs to assess intervention effectiveness in rehabilitation: A practical guide. Ann Phys Rehabil Med. 2018;61:164–79.CrossRefPubMed
64.
Zurück zum Zitat Bonin P, Peereman R, Malardier N, Méot A, Chalard M. A new set of 299 pictures for psycholinguistic studies: French norms for name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, age of acquisition, and naming latencies. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2003;35:158–67.CrossRefPubMed Bonin P, Peereman R, Malardier N, Méot A, Chalard M. A new set of 299 pictures for psycholinguistic studies: French norms for name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, age of acquisition, and naming latencies. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. 2003;35:158–67.CrossRefPubMed
65.
Zurück zum Zitat Tate RL, Perdices M, Rosenkoetter U, Wakim D, Godbee K, Togher L. Revision of a method quality rating scale for single-case experimental designs and n-of-1 trials: The 15-item Risk of Bias in N-of-1 Trials (RoBiNT) Scale. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2013;23:619–38.CrossRefPubMed Tate RL, Perdices M, Rosenkoetter U, Wakim D, Godbee K, Togher L. Revision of a method quality rating scale for single-case experimental designs and n-of-1 trials: The 15-item Risk of Bias in N-of-1 Trials (RoBiNT) Scale. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2013;23:619–38.CrossRefPubMed
66.
Zurück zum Zitat Tate RL, Perdices M, Rosenkoetter U, Shadish W, Vohra S, Barlow DH, et al. The Single-Case Reporting Guideline In BEhavioural Interventions (SCRIBE) 2016 Statement †. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2017;27:1–15.CrossRefPubMed Tate RL, Perdices M, Rosenkoetter U, Shadish W, Vohra S, Barlow DH, et al. The Single-Case Reporting Guideline In BEhavioural Interventions (SCRIBE) 2016 Statement †. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2017;27:1–15.CrossRefPubMed
67.
Zurück zum Zitat Kratochwill T, Levin J. Enhancing the Scientific Credibility of Single-Case Intervention Research: Randomization to the Rescue. Psychol Methods. 2010;15:124–44.CrossRefPubMed Kratochwill T, Levin J. Enhancing the Scientific Credibility of Single-Case Intervention Research: Randomization to the Rescue. Psychol Methods. 2010;15:124–44.CrossRefPubMed
68.
Zurück zum Zitat Lane JD, Gast DL. Visual analysis in single case experimental design studies: Brief review and guidelines. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2014;24:445–63.CrossRefPubMed Lane JD, Gast DL. Visual analysis in single case experimental design studies: Brief review and guidelines. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2014;24:445–63.CrossRefPubMed
69.
Zurück zum Zitat Parker RI, Vannest KJ, Davis JL, Sauber SB. Combining Nonoverlap and Trend for Single-Case Research: Tau-U. Behav Ther. 2011;42:284–99.CrossRefPubMed Parker RI, Vannest KJ, Davis JL, Sauber SB. Combining Nonoverlap and Trend for Single-Case Research: Tau-U. Behav Ther. 2011;42:284–99.CrossRefPubMed
70.
Zurück zum Zitat Tarlow KR. An Improved Rank Correlation Effect Size Statistic for Single-Case Designs: Baseline Corrected Tau. Behav Modif. 2017;41:427–67.CrossRefPubMed Tarlow KR. An Improved Rank Correlation Effect Size Statistic for Single-Case Designs: Baseline Corrected Tau. Behav Modif. 2017;41:427–67.CrossRefPubMed
72.
Zurück zum Zitat Byiers Breanne J, Joe R, J SF. Single-Subject Experimental Design for Evidence-Based Practice. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2012;21:397–414. Byiers Breanne J, Joe R, J SF. Single-Subject Experimental Design for Evidence-Based Practice. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2012;21:397–414.
Metadaten
Titel
Effects of a developmental dyslexia remediation protocol based on the training of audio-phonological cognitive processes in dyslexic children with high intellectual potential: study protocol for a multiple-baseline single-case experimental design
verfasst von
Gaëlle Darrot
Auriane Gros
Valeria Manera
Bruno De Cara
Sylvane Faure
Xavier Corveleyn
Karine Harrar-Eskinazi
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2023
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
BMC Pediatrics / Ausgabe 1/2023
Elektronische ISSN: 1471-2431
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-023-04189-6

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2023

BMC Pediatrics 1/2023 Zur Ausgabe

Ähnliche Überlebensraten nach Reanimation während des Transports bzw. vor Ort

29.05.2024 Reanimation im Kindesalter Nachrichten

Laut einer Studie aus den USA und Kanada scheint es bei der Reanimation von Kindern außerhalb einer Klinik keinen Unterschied für das Überleben zu machen, ob die Wiederbelebungsmaßnahmen während des Transports in die Klinik stattfinden oder vor Ort ausgeführt werden. Jedoch gibt es dabei einige Einschränkungen und eine wichtige Ausnahme.

Alter der Mutter beeinflusst Risiko für kongenitale Anomalie

28.05.2024 Kinder- und Jugendgynäkologie Nachrichten

Welchen Einfluss das Alter ihrer Mutter auf das Risiko hat, dass Kinder mit nicht chromosomal bedingter Malformation zur Welt kommen, hat eine ungarische Studie untersucht. Sie zeigt: Nicht nur fortgeschrittenes Alter ist riskant.

Begünstigt Bettruhe der Mutter doch das fetale Wachstum?

Ob ungeborene Kinder, die kleiner als die meisten Gleichaltrigen sind, schneller wachsen, wenn die Mutter sich mehr ausruht, wird diskutiert. Die Ergebnisse einer US-Studie sprechen dafür.

Bei Amblyopie früher abkleben als bisher empfohlen?

22.05.2024 Fehlsichtigkeit Nachrichten

Bei Amblyopie ist das frühzeitige Abkleben des kontralateralen Auges in den meisten Fällen wohl effektiver als der Therapiestandard mit zunächst mehrmonatigem Brilletragen.

Update Pädiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.