Skip to main content
Erschienen in: Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 1/2016

Open Access 01.12.2016 | Letter to the Editor

Short report: self-reported psychopathic traits in Finnish and Dutch samples of non-referred adolescents: exploration of cultural differences

verfasst von: Svetlana Oshukova, Riittakerttu Kaltiala-Heino, Sanne Hillege, Corine de Ruiter, Grigori Joffe, Jouko Miettunen, Riikka Marttila, Mauri Marttunen, Matti Kaivosoja, Nina Lindberg

Erschienen in: Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health | Ausgabe 1/2016

Abstract

Background

Culture-related differences in psychopathic traits have been reported for adults, but for adolescents such knowledge is lacking. The aim of this cross-national study was to compare self-reported psychopathic traits between Finnish and Dutch samples of mid-adolescent community youth.

Methods

The Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory (YPI) was filled in by 372 Finnish and 474 Dutch 15- to 16-year-old pupils. As gender-specific differences exist in psychopathic traits, we analyzed the data separately for boys and girls.

Results

Dutch boys scored significantly higher than Finnish boys on total and all dimensional scores of the YPI as well as on most sub-dimensional scores. Dutch girls scored significantly higher than Finnish girls on the Affective dimension and on the two corresponding sub-dimensions: remorselessness and callousness. Finnish girls scored significantly higher on grandiosity, which loads to the Interpersonal dimension of the YPI.

Conclusions

Our findings suggest that culture influences the manifestation of psychopathic traits already in adolescence and that this relation is more prominent in boys.

Background

Psychopathy is a constellation of interpersonal (dishonest charm, grandiosity, lying, and manipulative behavior), affective (remorselessness, unemotionality, and callousness), and behavioral (thrill-seeking, impulsivity, and irresponsibility) character traits [1]. Current conceptualizations see psychopathic traits on a dimensional continuum, where psychopathy is a malicious version of the extremes of normal personality traits [2]. Psychopathic traits are relatively stable over time, from childhood through adolescence to adulthood [3]. The gold standard for assessing adolescent psychopathic traits is the Psychopathy Checklist-Youth Version (PCL-YV) [4], which is an adaptation of the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised [5], designed for adults. The PCL-YV is, however, a time-consuming method that demands rigorous training and is mainly used in forensic samples. Because of this, various self-assessments have been widely used to measure psychopathic traits. Self-assessments are also cost effective to screen large samples. However, lack of valid cut-off points and valid reference groups limit their use in clinical practice.
In light of current literature, psychopathy is strongly associated with genetic and neurobiological background [68], but environmental factors also have an influence on its development [9]. Culture refers to the set of socially constructed and learned norms, values, beliefs, and behaviors shared by a group of individuals. Among adults, a recent community sample study covering 58 nations and more than 33,000 adults found that males and females from Western Europe produced the highest scores on the affective facet of the Self-Report Psychopathy scale, while individuals from Northern Europe tended to exhibit the lowest scores on both the affective and interpersonal facets [10]. With regard to juvenile psychopathy, as far, the main priority in psychopathy research has been to establish the validity of existing conceptualizations of juvenile psychopathy rather than to examine the construct in relation to culture. However, culture can influence juvenile antisocial behavior by operating through larger social systems [11]. Although psychopathy and antisocial behavior are distinct constructs, individuals with elevated psychopathic traits often exhibit disruptive behavior [12], and the behavioral component of the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised [5] highly reflects antisocial lifestyle.
The aim of this cross-national study was to compare psychopathic traits between Finnish and Dutch samples of 15- to 16-year-old non-referred youth. As gender-specific differences exist in psychopathic traits [13], we analyzed the data separately for boys and girls. We hypothesized that, in line with the recent study by Neumann et al. [10] among adults, Dutch adolescents representing Western Europe, would exhibit higher levels of psychopathy than Finnish adolescents representing Northern Europe.

Methods

Subjects

The Finnish sample

The sample comprised adolescents attending the 9th grade at five Finnish-speaking secondary schools in Kokkola city with approximately 47,000 citizens, on the western coast of Finland. Of the 446 students, 60 (13.4 %) did not participate in the study because of either failing to attend school on the study day or refusing to participate. Of the remaining 386 students, five did not complete the self-assessment and six did not answer the questions on general background variables, and thus, were excluded. From the 375 adolescents, those who were aged 15–16 years at the time of the assessment were selected. Thus, the final sample comprised 372 adolescents, 174 boys (46.8 %) and 198 girls (53.2 %), with a mean age of 15.1 years (SD = 0.27). For details, see [13].

The Dutch sample

The original sample comprised 776 adolescents in the upper grades of two secondary schools in two rural areas of the Netherlands. However, 36 adolescents (4.6 %) did not complete the questionnaire. From 740 pupils, those of foreign origin (e.g., Somalian, Turkish, Netherlands-Antillean) (n = 74, 10.0 %) as well as those younger than 15 years or older than 16 years (n = 238, 32.2 %) were removed from the sample. Thus, the final sample included 474 adolescents, 221 boys (46.6 %) and 253 girls (53.4 %), with a mean age of 15.4 years (SD = 0.49). For details, see [14].

Procedure

The adolescents filled in the self-assessment during their school lessons. Prior to completing the assessments, they received both oral and written information about the study. In Finland, return of the completed questionnaires by the participants was taken as confirmation of their consent. Privacy was ensured by having no identifying factors in the questionnaires, only age and gender were collected as background variables. In the Netherlands, written informed consent was obtained from all participants. In both countries, a letter was sent to the guardians of the students to inform them about the study. The study was approved by the local ethics boards and the administration of the schools.

The Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory (YPI)

The Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory (YPI) by Andershed et al. [15] was used to measure psychopathic traits. It assesses each psychopathic trait with several items, which are composed to tap psychopathic traits indirectly, framing the psychopathic features as abilities rather than deficits (e.g. “I usually feel calm when other people are scared” instead of “My emotions are shallow”). It consists altogether 50 statements scored on a 4-point Likert scale with response options ranging from “Does not apply at all = 1” to “Applies very well = 4”; thus, the total score of the scale can range from 50 to 200, with a higher score representing a higher level of the trait. The YPI has three dimensions and 10 sub-dimensions. The Interpersonal (Grandiose-manipulative) dimension contains sub-dimensions termed Dishonest charm, Grandiosity, Lying, and Manipulation, the Affective (Callous-unemotional) dimension contains Remorselessness, Unemotionality, and Callousness, and the Behavioral (Impulsive-irresponsible) dimension contains Thrill-seeking, Impulsiveness, and Irresponsibility. The original YPI showed internal consistencies ranging from marginal (Callousness: Cronbach’s alpha = 0.66; Unemotionality: 0.67) to acceptable and good (0.71–0.82) [15]. Later, and with various language versions, the YPI has shown moderate to good psychometric properties both in general [13, 14, 16, 17] and in forensic samples [18, 19]. In this study, we used the authorized Finnish [13] and Dutch [14] versions of the YPI. Both researcher groups used the English version of the YPI as a basis and performed the translation according to the recommendations of the developers (http://​www.​oru.​se/​jps/​downloadYPI/​) including an iterative process of translation and independent back translation, followed by a discussion to resolve minor differences.

Statistical analyses

In order to evaluate the internal consistency of the YPI, we calculated Cronbach’s alphas for the total, dimensional and sub-dimensional scores in both samples. In line with previous research, reliability coefficients of <0.60 were interpreted as insufficient, 0.60–0.69 as marginal, 0.70–0.79 as acceptable, 0.80–0.89 as good, and ≥0.90 as excellent [20].
The Mann–Whitney U test was used to compare the YPI scores because the data was skewed. Both average and median continuous scores are reported in line with previous research [1315, 21]. We used Cohen’s d to estimate the effect sizes of the differences, interpreting an effect size of 0.2–0.5 as small, 0.5–0.8 as medium, and >0.8 as large [20].
All statistical analyses were performed with IBM SPSS Statistics version 22.

Results

Internal consistency

In both genders, for the total score as well as for both the Interpersonal and Behavioral dimension score of the YPI, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients indicated good to excellent internal consistencies in the Finnish sample, but acceptable to good in the Dutch one. For the Affective dimension score, internal consistency was insufficient in Finnish boys, marginal in Dutch adolescents, and acceptable in Finnish girls. The internal consistencies were at least acceptable for most of the sub-dimensions, but there were some exceptions. The sub-dimension Callousness showed insufficient internal consistency in both samples across gender, and in the Dutch sample, Cronbach’s alpha coefficients revealed insufficient internal consistencies for the sub-dimension Unemotionality in both genders and for the sub-dimension Thrill-seeking in boys (Table 1).
Table 1
Internal consistencies (Cronbach’s alphas, α), descriptives, and group differences for the sub-dimensional, dimensional and the total scores of the Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory (YPI) in 15- to 16-year-old Finnish and Dutch boys and girls
 
Internal consistency
Descriptives and group differences
 
Finnish boys
Dutch boys
Finnish boys (n = 174)
Dutch boys (n = 221)
Statistics
α
α
Mean (SD)
Median
Mean (SD)
Median
U
p
Cohen’s d
YPI sub-dimension
 Dishonest charm
0.81
0.74
1.81 (0.67)
1.80
2.02 (0.65)
2.00
15,325.50
0.001
−0.318
 Grandiosity
0.79
0.81
1.89 (0.68)
1.80
1.86 (0.77)
1.75
18,074.50
NS
0.041
 Lying
0.82
0.72
1.80 (0.67)
1.80
2.01 (0.65)
2.00
15,343.00
0.001
−0.318
 Manipulation
0.86
0.81
1.70 (0.69)
1.60
1.98 (0.71)
1.80
14,399.00
<0.001
−0.440
 Remorselessness
0.83
0.63
1.69 (0.64)
1.60
2.01 (0.61)
2.00
12,963.00
<0.001
−0.440
 Unemotionality
0.65
0.50
2.15 (0.63)
2.20
2.24 (0.55)
2.20
1741.00
NS
−0.152
 Callousness
0.41
0.25
2.23 (0.51)
2.20
2.52 (0.46)
2.40
13,308.50
<0.001
−0.440
 Thrill-seeking
0.80
0.55
2.55 (0.72)
2.60
2.78 (0.57)
2.80
15,541.50
0.001
−0.353
 Impulsiveness
0.73
0.62
2.14 (0.67)
2.00
2.37 (0.62)
2.40
15,298.00
<0.001
−0.440
 Irresponsibility
0.74
0.60
1.85 (0.70)
1.80
1.85 (0.67)
1.80
18,782.00
NS
0.000
YPI dimension
 Interpersonal
0.90
0.85
7.19 (2.37)
7.00
7.88 (2.31)
7.40
15,686.50
0.002
−0.294
 Affective
0.55
0.61
6.07 (1.30)
6.00
6.77 (1.24)
6.80
12,756.50
<0.001
−0.440
 Behavioral
0.86
0.71
6.54 (1.84)
6.35
7.01 (1.44)
6.80
15,886.50
0.003
−0.285
 YPI Total
0.84
0.74
19.81 (4.91)
19.60
21.66 (4.18)
21.25
14,360.00
<0.001
−0.440
 
Finnish girls
Dutch girls
Finnish girls (n = 198)
Dutch girls (n = 253)
Statistics
α
α
Mean (SD)
Median
Mean (SD)
Median
U
p
Cohen’s d
YPI sub-dimension
 Dishonest charm
0.82
0.75
1.72 (0.66)
1.60
1.72 (0.60)
1.60
24,243.00
NS
0.000
 Grandiosity
0.82
0.76
1.58 (0.61)
1.40
1.34 (0.48)
1.20
18,940.00
<0.001
0.437
 Lying
0.77
0.78
1.64 (0.60)
1.40
1.64 (0.62)
1.40
24,980.50
NS
0.000
 Manipulation
0.81
0.77
1.62 (0.63)
1.40
1.57 (0.57)
1.40
24,491.50
NS
0.083
 Remorselessness
0.78
0.63
1.43 (0.52)
1.33
1.59 (0.52)
1.40
19,669.50
<0.001
−0.308
 Unemotionality
0.65
0.57
1.79 (0.54)
1.70
1.71 (0.47)
1.60
23,036.00
NS
0.158
 Callousness
0.44
0.46
1.67 (0.48)
1.60
1.89 (0.52)
1.80
18,583.00
<0.001
−0.440
 Thrill-seeking
0.78
0.72
2.47 (0.65)
2.55
2.51 (0.64)
2.40
24,538.00
NS
−0.062
 Impulsiveness
0.77
0.71
2.20 (0.67)
2.20
2.32 (0.64)
2.20
22,805.50
NS
0.183
 Irresponsibility
0.68
0.59
1.60 (0.57)
1.40
1.56 (0.51)
1.40
24,746.00
NS
0.074
YPI dimension
 Interpersonal
0.87
0.83
6.56 (2.12)
6.20
6.28 (1.86)
5.80
23,538.00
NS
0.140
 Affective
0.73
0.61
4.89 (1.24)
4.60
5.18 (1.11)
5.00
20,389.00
0.001
−0.246
 Behavioral
0.80
0.76
6.27 (1.59)
6.20
6.39 (1.47)
6.20
23,852.00
NS
−0.078
 YPI Total
0.77
0.76
17.71 (4.18)
17.10
17.84 (3.72)
17.20
24,342.00
NS
−0.032
Comparisons are performed using the Mann–Whitney U test. Effect sizes are reported
NS not statistically significant

Psychopathy scores

The Dutch boys scored significantly higher than the Finnish boys on the YPI total score and all dimensional scores as well as on sub-dimensional scores except Grandiosity, Unemotionality, and Irresponsibility, where no significant differences were observed (Table 1; Fig. 1). The Dutch girls scored significantly higher than their Finnish counterparts on the Affective dimension score and on the two corresponding sub-dimensions: Remorselessness and Callousness. The Finnish girls, on the other hand, scored significantly higher on Grandiosity.

Discussion

Most previous studies on the association of ethnicity and culture with adolescent psychopathic traits have focused on differences across race [22, 23] or between Europeans and ethnic minorities, mostly among offender samples [24, 25]. There has been some evidence that African-American youth tend to score higher on psychopathic traits than European-American youth [22], and that psychopathic traits influence criminal behavior in different ways across race [23]. On the other hand, neither Pechorro et al. [24] nor Veen et al. [25] found differences in manifestations of psychopathic traits between ethnic groups. To our knowledge, this study is the first to compare community youth of two European countries on level of psychopathic traits.
Dutch boys exhibited significantly higher psychopathic traits than their Finnish peers. With regard to dimensions, they also showed higher levels of affective and interpersonal traits of the psychopathy construct. Dutch girls, on the other hand, scored significantly higher than Finnish girls only on the Affective dimension. This finding is interesting, since, according to many researchers, it is interpersonal and affective features that comprise the “core” of the psychopathic character, and the behavioral component could only be seen as a consequence of the syndrome [26]. Our findings definitely concur with those from a recent world-wide community research by Neumann et al. [10] among adults , and suggest, as we hypothesized, that culture- related differences in psychopathic traits can be detected already in adolescents.
Dutch boys also scored significantly higher than Finnish boys on the behavioral component of psychopathic traits, reflecting impulsive and irresponsible lifestyle, while among girls this difference was not observed. Interestingly, Pickett et al. [27] explored self-reported physical fighting among school boys and girls in 35 different countries. Involvement of boys in physical fighting during the previous year was lowest in Finland (36.7 %), while the Netherlands ranked 9th (50.5 %). However, Dutch girls also reported being involved in fighting more often than their Finnish peers (22.1 vs. 13.3 %), which is not in line with our results.
An obvious strength of this study was that we were able to collect two samples of mid-adolescent community youth evaluated with an instrument (YPI) widely used in psychopathy research with adolescent populations. However, the samples were not collected simultaneously, but as two individual study projects: the Dutch-sample was collected in 2005 and the Finnish one in 2014. Since the Finns, contrary to the Dutch, are an ethnically extremely homogeneous population, we excluded adolescents of foreign origin. Although the YPI has often been used in community youth, concern has risen on its ability to measure the affective component of the psychopathy construct [21]. In line with many previous community studies [1517], we found especially low internal consistency coefficients for the Unemotionality and Callousness sub-scales. It has been proposed that these sub-scales contain too few items to permit adequate scale reliabilities and that the YPI should be revised in the future [21]. So, weak scale reliabilities of some sub-scales observed in our study rather reflect the known weakness of the questionnaire than indicate that the translated versions might not be culture-adequate. Also, the effect sizes were small pointing to a sufficient content validity of the translated YPI versions and, to some extent, a true culture-effect.

Conclusions

Our cross-national study suggests culture-related differences in juvenile psychopathic traits. This preliminary research should obviously be replicated with other cross-national samples; if significant differences emerge, the YPI as well as other self-report questionnaires for psychopathic traits might need nation-specific reference values. At present, some caution is needed in generalizing the national research findings.

Authors’ contributions

SO organized and analyzed the data and served as the first author. JM and RM participated in performing the statistical analyses. RK-H, SH, CR, GJ, MM, MK and NL participated in the writing process. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge the authorities of Helsinki University Hospital, Department of Psychiatry and Kellokoski Hospital.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Open AccessThis article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://​creativecommons.​org/​publicdomain/​zero/​1.​0/​) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
Literatur
1.
Zurück zum Zitat Hare RD. Psychopathy, affect, and behavior. In: Cooke DJ, Forth AE, Hare RD, editors. Psychopathy: theory, research, and implications for society. Dordrecht: Kluwer; 1998. p. 105–38.CrossRef Hare RD. Psychopathy, affect, and behavior. In: Cooke DJ, Forth AE, Hare RD, editors. Psychopathy: theory, research, and implications for society. Dordrecht: Kluwer; 1998. p. 105–38.CrossRef
2.
Zurück zum Zitat Benning SD, Patrick CJ, Blonigen DM, Hicks BM, Iacono WG. Estimating facets of psychopathy from normal personality traits: a step toward community epidemiological investigations. Assessment. 2005;12:3–18.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Benning SD, Patrick CJ, Blonigen DM, Hicks BM, Iacono WG. Estimating facets of psychopathy from normal personality traits: a step toward community epidemiological investigations. Assessment. 2005;12:3–18.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
3.
Zurück zum Zitat Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE. Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and the treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychol Bull. 2014;40:1–57.CrossRef Frick PJ, Ray JV, Thornton LC, Kahn RE. Can callous-unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis, and the treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychol Bull. 2014;40:1–57.CrossRef
4.
Zurück zum Zitat Forth AE, Kosson DS, Hare RD. The Hare PCL: YV. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems; 2003. Forth AE, Kosson DS, Hare RD. The Hare PCL: YV. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems; 2003.
5.
Zurück zum Zitat Hare RD. The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems; 1991. Hare RD. The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems; 1991.
6.
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson NE, Kiehl KA. Psychopathy: developmental perspectives and their implications for treatment. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2014;32:103–17.PubMedPubMedCentral Anderson NE, Kiehl KA. Psychopathy: developmental perspectives and their implications for treatment. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2014;32:103–17.PubMedPubMedCentral
7.
Zurück zum Zitat Tuvblad C, Bezdjian S, Raine A, Baker LA. The heritability of psychopathic personality in 14- to 15-year-old twins: a multirater, multimeasure approach. Psychol Assess. 2014;26:704–16.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Tuvblad C, Bezdjian S, Raine A, Baker LA. The heritability of psychopathic personality in 14- to 15-year-old twins: a multirater, multimeasure approach. Psychol Assess. 2014;26:704–16.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
8.
Zurück zum Zitat Viding E, McCrory EJ. Genetic and neurocognitive contributions to the development of psychopathy. Dev Psychopathol. 2012;24:969–83.PubMedCrossRef Viding E, McCrory EJ. Genetic and neurocognitive contributions to the development of psychopathy. Dev Psychopathol. 2012;24:969–83.PubMedCrossRef
9.
Zurück zum Zitat Daversa MT. Early environmental predictors of the affective and interpersonal constructs of psychopathy. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol. 2010;54:6–21.PubMedCrossRef Daversa MT. Early environmental predictors of the affective and interpersonal constructs of psychopathy. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol. 2010;54:6–21.PubMedCrossRef
10.
Zurück zum Zitat Neumann CS, Schmitt DS, Carter R, Embley I, Hare RD. Psychopathic traits in females and males across the globe. Behav Sci Law. 2012;30:557–74.PubMedCrossRef Neumann CS, Schmitt DS, Carter R, Embley I, Hare RD. Psychopathic traits in females and males across the globe. Behav Sci Law. 2012;30:557–74.PubMedCrossRef
11.
Zurück zum Zitat Verona E, Sadeh N, Javdani S. The influence of gender and culture on child and adolescent psychopathy. In: Salekin R, Lynam D, editors. Handbook of child and adolescent psychopathy. New York: The Guilford Press; 2010. p. 317–42. Verona E, Sadeh N, Javdani S. The influence of gender and culture on child and adolescent psychopathy. In: Salekin R, Lynam D, editors. Handbook of child and adolescent psychopathy. New York: The Guilford Press; 2010. p. 317–42.
12.
Zurück zum Zitat Patrick CJ. Conceptualizing the psychopathic personality: disinhibited, bold,… Or just plain mean? In: Salekin R, Lynam D, editors. Handbook of child and adolescent psychopathy. New York: The Guilford Press; 2010. Patrick CJ. Conceptualizing the psychopathic personality: disinhibited, bold,… Or just plain mean? In: Salekin R, Lynam D, editors. Handbook of child and adolescent psychopathy. New York: The Guilford Press; 2010.
13.
Zurück zum Zitat Oshukova S, Kaltiala-Heino R, Miettunen J, Marttila R, Tani P, Aronen ET, Marttunen M, Kaivosoja M, Lindberg N. Self-reported psychopathic traits among non-referred Finnish adolescents: psychometric properties of the Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory and the Antisocial Process Screening Device. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2015;9:15.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Oshukova S, Kaltiala-Heino R, Miettunen J, Marttila R, Tani P, Aronen ET, Marttunen M, Kaivosoja M, Lindberg N. Self-reported psychopathic traits among non-referred Finnish adolescents: psychometric properties of the Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory and the Antisocial Process Screening Device. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. 2015;9:15.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
14.
Zurück zum Zitat Hillege S, Das J, de Ruiter C. The Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory: psychometric properties and its relation to substance use and interpersonal style in a Dutch sample of non-referred adolescents. J Adolesc. 2010;33:83–91.PubMedCrossRef Hillege S, Das J, de Ruiter C. The Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory: psychometric properties and its relation to substance use and interpersonal style in a Dutch sample of non-referred adolescents. J Adolesc. 2010;33:83–91.PubMedCrossRef
15.
Zurück zum Zitat Andershed H, Kerr M, Stattin H, Levander S. Psychopathic traits in non- referred youths: initial test of a new assessment tool. In: Blaauw E, Sheridan L, editors. Psychopaths: current international perspectives. Den Haag: Elsevier; 2002. p. 131–58. Andershed H, Kerr M, Stattin H, Levander S. Psychopathic traits in non- referred youths: initial test of a new assessment tool. In: Blaauw E, Sheridan L, editors. Psychopaths: current international perspectives. Den Haag: Elsevier; 2002. p. 131–58.
16.
Zurück zum Zitat Declercq F, Markey S, Vandist K, Verhaeghe P. The Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory: factor structure and antisocial behaviour in non-referred 12- to 17-year-olds. J Forens Psychiatry Psychol. 2009;20:577–94.CrossRef Declercq F, Markey S, Vandist K, Verhaeghe P. The Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory: factor structure and antisocial behaviour in non-referred 12- to 17-year-olds. J Forens Psychiatry Psychol. 2009;20:577–94.CrossRef
17.
Zurück zum Zitat Larsson H, Andershed H, Lichtenstein P. A genetic factor explains most of the variation in the psychopathic personality. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006;115:221–30.PubMedCrossRef Larsson H, Andershed H, Lichtenstein P. A genetic factor explains most of the variation in the psychopathic personality. J Abnorm Psychol. 2006;115:221–30.PubMedCrossRef
18.
Zurück zum Zitat Poythress NG, Dembo R, Wareham J, Greenbaum PE. Construct validity of the Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory (YPI) and the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD) with justice involved adolescents. Crim Justice Behav. 2006;33:26–55.CrossRef Poythress NG, Dembo R, Wareham J, Greenbaum PE. Construct validity of the Youth Psychopathic traits Inventory (YPI) and the Antisocial Process Screening Device (APSD) with justice involved adolescents. Crim Justice Behav. 2006;33:26–55.CrossRef
19.
Zurück zum Zitat Dolan MC, Rennie CE. The relationship between psychopathic traits measured by the Youth psychopathic trait Inventory and psychopathology in a UK sample of conduct disordered boys. J Adolesc. 2007;30:601–11.PubMedCrossRef Dolan MC, Rennie CE. The relationship between psychopathic traits measured by the Youth psychopathic trait Inventory and psychopathology in a UK sample of conduct disordered boys. J Adolesc. 2007;30:601–11.PubMedCrossRef
21.
Zurück zum Zitat Andershed H, Hodgins S, Tengström A. Convergent validity of the Youth psychopathic traits Inventory (YPI): association with the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version (PCL-YV). Assessment. 2007;14:144–54.PubMedCrossRef Andershed H, Hodgins S, Tengström A. Convergent validity of the Youth psychopathic traits Inventory (YPI): association with the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version (PCL-YV). Assessment. 2007;14:144–54.PubMedCrossRef
22.
Zurück zum Zitat McCoy WK, Edens JF. Do black and white youths differ in levels of psychopathic traits? A meta-analysis of the psychopathy checklist measures. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2006;74:386–92.PubMedCrossRef McCoy WK, Edens JF. Do black and white youths differ in levels of psychopathic traits? A meta-analysis of the psychopathy checklist measures. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2006;74:386–92.PubMedCrossRef
23.
Zurück zum Zitat Thornton LC, Frick PJ, Shulman EP, Ray JV, Steinberg L, Cauffman E. Callous-unemotional traits and adolescents’ role in group crime. Law Hum Behav. 2015;39:368–77.PubMedCrossRef Thornton LC, Frick PJ, Shulman EP, Ray JV, Steinberg L, Cauffman E. Callous-unemotional traits and adolescents’ role in group crime. Law Hum Behav. 2015;39:368–77.PubMedCrossRef
24.
Zurück zum Zitat Pechorro P, Maroco J, Poiares C, Vieira RX. Validation of the Portuguese version of the antisocial process screening device-self-report with a focus on delinquent behavior and behavior problems. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol. 2013;57:112–26.PubMedCrossRef Pechorro P, Maroco J, Poiares C, Vieira RX. Validation of the Portuguese version of the antisocial process screening device-self-report with a focus on delinquent behavior and behavior problems. Int J Offender Ther Comp Criminol. 2013;57:112–26.PubMedCrossRef
25.
Zurück zum Zitat Veen VC, Stevens GW, Andershed H, Raaijmakers QA, Doreleijers TA, Vollebergh WA. Cross-ethnic generalizability of the three-factor model of psychopathy: the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory in an incarcerated sample of native Dutch and Moroccan immigrant boys. Int J Law Psychiatry. 2011;34:127–30.PubMedCrossRef Veen VC, Stevens GW, Andershed H, Raaijmakers QA, Doreleijers TA, Vollebergh WA. Cross-ethnic generalizability of the three-factor model of psychopathy: the Youth Psychopathic Traits Inventory in an incarcerated sample of native Dutch and Moroccan immigrant boys. Int J Law Psychiatry. 2011;34:127–30.PubMedCrossRef
26.
Zurück zum Zitat Cooke DJ, Michie C, Hart SD, Clark DA. Reconstructing psychopathy: clarifying the significance of antisocial and socially deviant behavior in the diagnosis of psychopathic personality disorder. J Pers Disord. 2004;18:337–57.PubMedCrossRef Cooke DJ, Michie C, Hart SD, Clark DA. Reconstructing psychopathy: clarifying the significance of antisocial and socially deviant behavior in the diagnosis of psychopathic personality disorder. J Pers Disord. 2004;18:337–57.PubMedCrossRef
27.
Zurück zum Zitat Pickett W, Craig W, Harel Y, Cunningham J, Simpson K, Molcho M, Mazur J, Dostaler S, Overpeck MD, Currie CE. HBSC Violence and Injuries Writing Group. Cross-national study of fighting and weapon carrying as determinants of adolescent injury. Pediatrics. 2005;116:e855–63.PubMedCrossRef Pickett W, Craig W, Harel Y, Cunningham J, Simpson K, Molcho M, Mazur J, Dostaler S, Overpeck MD, Currie CE. HBSC Violence and Injuries Writing Group. Cross-national study of fighting and weapon carrying as determinants of adolescent injury. Pediatrics. 2005;116:e855–63.PubMedCrossRef
Metadaten
Titel
Short report: self-reported psychopathic traits in Finnish and Dutch samples of non-referred adolescents: exploration of cultural differences
verfasst von
Svetlana Oshukova
Riittakerttu Kaltiala-Heino
Sanne Hillege
Corine de Ruiter
Grigori Joffe
Jouko Miettunen
Riikka Marttila
Mauri Marttunen
Matti Kaivosoja
Nina Lindberg
Publikationsdatum
01.12.2016
Verlag
BioMed Central
Erschienen in
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health / Ausgabe 1/2016
Elektronische ISSN: 1753-2000
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1186/s13034-015-0090-3

Weitere Artikel der Ausgabe 1/2016

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health 1/2016 Zur Ausgabe

Hörschwäche erhöht Demenzrisiko unabhängig von Beta-Amyloid

29.05.2024 Hörstörungen Nachrichten

Hört jemand im Alter schlecht, nimmt das Hirn- und Hippocampusvolumen besonders schnell ab, was auch mit einem beschleunigten kognitiven Abbau einhergeht. Und diese Prozesse scheinen sich unabhängig von der Amyloidablagerung zu ereignen.

So wirken verschiedene Alkoholika auf den Blutdruck

23.05.2024 Störungen durch Alkohol Nachrichten

Je mehr Alkohol Menschen pro Woche trinken, desto mehr steigt ihr Blutdruck, legen Daten aus Dänemark nahe. Ob es dabei auch auf die Art des Alkohols ankommt, wurde ebenfalls untersucht.

„Übersichtlicher Wegweiser“: Lauterbachs umstrittener Klinik-Atlas ist online

17.05.2024 Klinik aktuell Nachrichten

Sie sei „ethisch geboten“, meint Gesundheitsminister Karl Lauterbach: mehr Transparenz über die Qualität von Klinikbehandlungen. Um sie abzubilden, lässt er gegen den Widerstand vieler Länder einen virtuellen Klinik-Atlas freischalten.

ADHS-Medikation erhöht das kardiovaskuläre Risiko

16.05.2024 Herzinsuffizienz Nachrichten

Erwachsene, die Medikamente gegen das Aufmerksamkeitsdefizit-Hyperaktivitätssyndrom einnehmen, laufen offenbar erhöhte Gefahr, an Herzschwäche zu erkranken oder einen Schlaganfall zu erleiden. Es scheint eine Dosis-Wirkungs-Beziehung zu bestehen.

Update Psychiatrie

Bestellen Sie unseren Fach-Newsletter und bleiben Sie gut informiert.